HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In
linguistics Linguistics is the science, scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure ...
(especially
generative grammar Generative grammar, or generativism , is a linguistic theory that regards linguistics as the study of a hypothesised innate grammatical structure. It is a biological or biologistic modification of earlier structuralist theories of linguisti ...
), complementizer or complementiser (
glossing abbreviation This article lists common abbreviations for grammatical terms that are used in linguistic interlinear glossing of oral languages in English. The list provides conventional glosses as established by standard inventories of glossing abbreviations su ...
: ) is a functional category (part of speech) that includes those words that can be used to turn a clause into the subject or
object Object may refer to: General meanings * Object (philosophy), a thing, being, or concept ** Object (abstract), an object which does not exist at any particular time or place ** Physical object, an identifiable collection of matter * Goal, an ...
of a sentence. For example, the word ''that'' may be called a complementizer in English sentences like ''Mary believes that it is raining''. The concept of complementizers is specific to certain modern grammatical theories; in traditional grammar, such words are normally considered conjunctions. The standard abbreviation for ''complementizer'' is C.


Category of C


C as head of CP

The complementizer is often held to be the syntactic head of a full clause, which is therefore often represented by the abbreviation CP (for ''complementizer
phrase In syntax and grammar, a phrase is a group of words or singular word acting as a grammatical unit. For instance, the English expression "the very happy squirrel" is a noun phrase which contains the adjective phrase "very happy". Phrases can consi ...
''). Evidence that the complementizer functions as the head of its clause includes that it is commonly the last element in a clause in
head-final In linguistics, head directionality is a proposed parameter that classifies languages according to whether they are head-initial (the head of a phrase precedes its complements) or head-final (the head follows its complements). The head is the ...
languages like
Korean Korean may refer to: People and culture * Koreans, ethnic group originating in the Korean Peninsula * Korean cuisine * Korean culture * Korean language **Korean alphabet, known as Hangul or Chosŏn'gŭl **Korean dialects and the Jeju language ** ...
or
Japanese Japanese may refer to: * Something from or related to Japan, an island country in East Asia * Japanese language, spoken mainly in Japan * Japanese people, the ethnic group that identifies with Japan through ancestry or culture ** Japanese diaspor ...
, in which other heads follow their
complement A complement is something that completes something else. Complement may refer specifically to: The arts * Complement (music), an interval that, when added to another, spans an octave ** Aggregate complementation, the separation of pitch-clas ...
s, whereas it appears at the start of a clause in
head-initial In linguistics, head directionality is a proposed parameter that classifies languages according to whether they are head-initial (the head of a phrase precedes its complements) or head-final (the head follows its complements). The head is the ...
languages such as English, where heads normally precede their complements. The trees below illustrate the phrase "Taro said that he married Hanako" in Japanese and English; syntactic heads are marked in red, demonstrating that C falls in head-final position in Japanese, and in head-initial position in English.


Sources of C

It is common for the complementizers of a language to develop historically from other syntactic categories, a process known as grammaticalization.


C can develop from a determiner

Across the languages of the world, it is especially common for
pronoun In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun (abbreviated ) is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase. Pronouns have traditionally been regarded as one of the parts of speech, but some modern theorists would not c ...
s or determiners to be used as complementizers (e.g., English ''that''). ''I read in the paper that it's going to be cold today.''


C can develop from an interrogative word

Another frequent source of complementizers is the class of interrogative words. It is especially common for a form that otherwise means ''what'' to be borrowed as a complementizer, but other interrogative words are often used as well, as in the following colloquial English example, where unstressed ''how'' is roughly equivalent to ''that''. ''I read in the paper how it's going to be cold today.''


C can develop from a preposition

With non-finite clauses, English ''for'' in sentences like ''I would prefer for there to be a table in the corner'' shows a preposition that has arguably developed into a complementizer. (The sequence ''for there'' in this sentence is not a
prepositional phrase An adpositional phrase, in linguistics, is a syntactic category that includes ''prepositional phrases'', ''postpositional phrases'', and ''circumpositional phrases''. Adpositional phrases contain an adposition (preposition, postposition, or ci ...
under this analysis.)


C can develop from a verb

In many languages of West Africa and South Asia, the form of the complementizer can be related to the verb ''say''. In these languages, the complementizer is also called the quotative. The quotative performs many extended functions in these languages.


Empty complementizers

Some analyses allow for the possibility of invisible or "empty" complementizers. An empty complementizer is considered to be present when there is not a word, even though the rules of grammar expect one. The complementizer (for example, "that") is usually said to be understood – ''i.e.'', an English speaker knows it is there, and so it does not need to be said. Its existence in English has been proposed based on the following type of alternation: :''He hopes you go ahead with the speech'' :''He hopes that you go ahead with the speech'' Because ''that'' can be inserted between the verb and the embedded clause without changing the meaning, the original sentence without a visible complementizer would be reanalyzed as :''He hopes ∅C you go ahead with the speech'' Where the symbol ''∅C'' represents the empty (or "null") complementizer, this suggests another interpretation of the earlier "how" sentence: :''I read in the paper C t's going to be cold today' where "how" serves as a specifier to the empty complementizer. This allows for a consistent analysis of another troublesome alternation: :''The man C saw yesterdayate my lunch!'' :''The man C saw yesterdayate my lunch!'' :''The man that saw yesterdayate my lunch!'' where "OP" represents an invisible interrogative known as an operator. In a more general sense, the proposed empty complementizer parallels the suggestion of near-universal empty determiners. Various analyses have been proposed to explain when the empty complementizer ''∅'' can substitute for a phonologically overt complementizer. One explanation is that complementizers are eligible for omission when they are epistemically neutral or redundant. For example, in many environments, English's epistemically neutral ''that'' and Danish's ''at'' can be omitted. In addition, if a complementizer expresses a semantic meaning that is also expressed by another marker in the phrase, the complementizer carrying the redundant meaning may be omitted. Consider the complementizer ''be'' in Mangap-Mbula, which expresses uncertainty, in the following example: Here, the marker ''ko'' also expresses epistemic uncertainty, so ''be'' can be replaced by the phonologically null complementizer without affecting meaning or grammaticality. Complementizers are present in a wide range of environments; in some, C is obligatorily overt (i.e., cannot be replaced by the empty complementizer). For example, in English, CPs selected for by manner-of-speaking verbs (''whisper, mutter, groan'', etc) resist C-drop: :''Barney whispered *(that) Wilma was dating Fred.'' :''Barney said (that) Wilma was dating Fred.'' In other environments, the complementizer can be omitted without loss of grammaticality, but may result in semantic ambiguity. For example, consider the English sentence ''"The newspaper reported that a new mayor was elected and'' (''that)'' ''there was a riot."'' Listeners can infer a causal relationship between the two events reported by the newspaper: a new mayor was elected, and as a result, there was a riot. Alternatively, the events may be interpreted as independent of each other. The non-causal interpretation is more likely when the second complementizer ''that'' is present, whereas the causal interpretation is more likely when an empty complementizer is present. The ambiguity here arises because the sentence where the second complementizer is empty may also be interpreted as simply having no second complementizer. In the first case, the sentence involves coordination of CPs, which lends itself more easily to a non-causal interpretation, but the latter case involves coordination of TPs, which is the necessary structure for a causal interpretation. Partial syntax trees for the possible structures are given below.


Selectional restrictions imposed by C

As a syntactic head, C always selects for a complement tense phrase (TP) whose syntax and semantics are dictated by the choice of C. The choice of C can determine whether the associated TP is finite or non-finite, whether it carries the semantic meaning of certainty or uncertainty, whether it expresses a question or an assertion, et cetera.


Propositions vs. indirect questions

The following complementizers are available in English: ''that, for, if, whether, ∅''. ''If'' and ''whether'' form CPs that express indirect questions: :''John wonders whether / if it is raining outside.'' In contrast, complementizers ''for, that,'' as well as the phonologically null complementizer ''∅'' introduce " declarative or non-interrogative" CPs. :''John thinks ∅ it is raining outside.'' :''John thinks that it is raining outside.'' :''John prefers for it to be raining.''


Finite vs. non-finite TPs

Tense phrases in English can be divided into finite (tensed) clauses or non-finite (tenseless) clauses. The former includes an indication of the relative time when its content occurs; the latter has no overt indication of time: compare ''John will leave'' (John's leaving will take place in the future) with ''John wants to leave'' (we are unsure when John is leaving). Certain complementizers strictly select for finite clauses (denoted
finite Finite is the opposite of infinite. It may refer to: * Finite number (disambiguation) * Finite set, a set whose cardinality (number of elements) is some natural number * Finite verb, a verb form that has a subject, usually being inflected or marke ...
while others select for non-finite clauses (denoted
finite Finite is the opposite of infinite. It may refer to: * Finite number (disambiguation) * Finite set, a set whose cardinality (number of elements) is some natural number * Finite verb, a verb form that has a subject, usually being inflected or marke ...
. Complementizers ''if, that'' require tenseTP: :''Mary wishes that she will win the game.'' (future) :''Mary believes if she wins the game, she can date John.'' (present) Complementizer ''for'' requires a tenseTP: :''Mary hopes for Kate to win the game.'' (infinitive) Complementizer ''whether'' allows either tenseor tenseTP: :''John wonders whether Mary will win the game.'' (future) :''Mary wonders whether to win the game or not.'' (infinitive)


Epistemic selection

Complementizers frequently carry epistemic meaning - that is, meaning about the speaker's degree of certainty (for example, whether they are doubtful), or the speaker's source of information (for example, whether they are making an inference, or have direct evidence). Contrast the meaning of "if" and "that" in English: :''John doesn't know if Mary is there.'' :''John doesn’t know that Mary is there.'' “If” signals that the associated tense phrase must carry the epistemic meaning of uncertainty. In contrast, “that” is epistemically neutral. This contrast is not uncommon cross-linguistically: in languages with only two complementizers, one is frequently neutral, while the other carries the meaning of uncertainty. One such language is Lango (a Nilotic language spoken in Uganda): Additional languages with this neutrality/uncertainty complementizer contrast include several European languages: In other languages, complementizers are richer in epistemic meaning. For example, in Mangap-Mbula, an Austronesian language of Papua New Guinea, the following complementizers are available: More generally, complementizers have been found to express the following values cross-linguistically: certainty, (general) uncertainty, probability, negative probability/falsehood, apprehension, and reportativity. Complementizers in Itzaj Maya also demonstrate epistemic meaning. For instance, English ''that'' and Itzaj Maya ''kej'' are used not only to identify complements, but also to introduce relative clauses: Itzaj Maya
Hofling, 2000
496, 495) (1) a. Taken fro
Boye (2015)
page 3 (1) b. Taken fro
Boye (2015)
page 3 (1a) introduces a subdordinate clause and (1b) introduces a conditional clause, similar to English. The former subtype ''that'' can be defined in terms of ''information source'' and includes meanings glossed as direct evidence, indirect evidence, hearsay, inferential. The latter one ''if'' can be defined in terms of ''degree of certainty'' and includes meanings glossed as certainty, probability, epistemic possibility, doubt. Thus, epistemic meaning as a whole can be defined in terms of the notion of ''justificatory support''. Abbreviations adapted from Boye (2015) page 15 an
Hofling (1998)
page 225-226.


Complementizer stacking

But not only that, Itzaj Maya can combine the neutral complementizer ''ke'' with the non-neutral, ''waj.'' This is illustrated in examples (2a), where the neutral complementizer ''ke'' occurs alone, and (2b), where it is optionally inserted in front of the uncertainty complementizer ''waj:'' Itzaj Maya
Hofling, 2000
495, 506) (2) a. Taken fro
Boye (2015)
page 9 (2) b. Taken fro
Boye (2015)
page 9 In (1a,b) and (2a) we saw each complementizer can be licensed once within the clause, but in (2b) we observe the significant difference of Itzaj Maya from English. In English we are able to license multiple C as long as the clause is completed with the embedded V or D. For example, I saw that fox that ran towards the garden that Tommy took care of. In such cases, C can appear as the complement of V or D many times. However, CP-recursion in two tiers or CP appearing as an immediate complement of maximal projection CP cannot be allowed in English. This action of ''Complementizer Stacking'' is realised as ungrammatical. In Scandinavian languages, however, we see this phenomenon of complementizer stacking. For example, researchers observed the two basic types of CP-recursion that occur independently in Danish: a CP with V2 (i.e. a CP headed by a lexical predicate in its head position) will be referred as CP ("big CP"), whereas a CP without V2 (i.e. CP headed by a non-lexical element) will be referred to as cP ("little cP"). * P_c°_[–_LEXICAL_("little_cP") *_[cP_C°_[+_LEXICAL.html" ;"title="_LEXICAL.html" ;"title="P c° [– LEXICAL">P c° [– LEXICAL ("little cP") * [cP C° [+ LEXICAL">_LEXICAL.html" ;"title="P c° [– LEXICAL">P c° [– LEXICAL ("little cP") * [cP C° [+ LEXICAL ("big CP") Adapted fro
Nyvad et al., (2017
page 453. The case of little/big CPs are comparable to "VP shell" structure in English where it introduces a small v in the higher position in the tree, and big V in the lower position in the tree. We also see in the examples, Danish allows complementizer stacking in constructions involving subject extraction from complement and relative clauses in colloquial speech: (3) a. (3) b. Taken fro
Nyvad et al., (2017)
page 463-464 We observe that complementizers are indeed stacked together in the beginning of the clause, acting as a complement of DP. CP-recusion structure on the right is applied for each of the clause, which points to evidence of complementizer stacking in Danish. In addition, we note the combination of ''som at der'' in (3a) is only possible in one specific order, which lead the researchers to believe that s''om'' may not require an empty operator in its Spec-CP position.


In various languages


Assyrian Neo-Aramaic

In Assyrian Neo-Aramaic, a modern Aramaic language, (or , depending on the dialect) is used as a complementizer and is related to the
relativizer In linguistics, a relativizer (abbreviated ) is a type of conjunction that introduces a relative clause. For example, in English, the conjunction ''that'' may be considered a relativizer in a sentence such as "I have one that you can use."Fox, Bar ...
. Though it is less common in casual speech, and more so in formal conversation.


Hebrew

In
Hebrew Hebrew (; ; ) is a Northwest Semitic language of the Afroasiatic language family. Historically, it is one of the spoken languages of the Israelites and their longest-surviving descendants, the Jews and Samaritans. It was largely preserved ...
(both Modern and Ancient) two complementizers coexist: שֶ , which is either related to the
relativizer In linguistics, a relativizer (abbreviated ) is a type of conjunction that introduces a relative clause. For example, in English, the conjunction ''that'' may be considered a relativizer in a sentence such as "I have one that you can use."Fox, Bar ...
''‘asher'' ( < Akkadian ''‘ashru'' ‘place’) and/or to the pronominal Proto-Semitic ''dhu'' ('this'); and כִּי i which is also used as a conjunction meaning 'because, when'. In modern usage, the latter is reserved for more formal writing.


American Sign Language

While some manual complementizers exist in American Sign Language they are usually expressed non-manually by grammaticalized
facial expressions A facial expression is one or more motions or positions of the muscles beneath the skin of the face. According to one set of controversial theories, these movements convey the emotional state of an individual to observers. Facial expressions are ...
.
Conditional clause Conditional sentences are natural language sentences that express that one thing is contingent on something else, e.g. "If it rains, the picnic will be cancelled." They are so called because the impact of the main clause of the sentence is ''con ...
s, for example, are indicated by raised eye-brows. If a manual complementizer is used it is also accompanied by a facial expression. The non-manual marking of complementizers is a common phenomenon found in many sign languages and it was even suggested that C-categories are universally marked with the face in sign languages.Bross, Fabian (2020): The clausal syntax of German Sign Language. A cartographic approach. Berlin: Language Science Press.


See also

* X-bar theory#Structure of S' * Dependent clause * ECM verb * That-clause


Notes


References

* Bjorkman, Bronwyn M. et al. “A syntactic answer to a pragmatic puzzle: The case of asymmetric and.” (2013). * Boye, K. (2012). Epistemic meaning. In ''Epistemic Meaning''. De Gruyter Mouton. * Boye, K., van Lier, E., & Theilgaard Brink, E. (2015). Epistemic complementizers: A cross-linguistic survey. ''Language Sciences (Oxford), 51''(Sep), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2015.04.001 * Hofling, C. A. (2000). ''Itzaj maya grammar''. University of Utah Press. * Kaltenböck, Gunther. “‘… That Is the Question': Complementizer Omission in Extraposed That-Clauses.” ''English Language and Linguistics'', vol. 10, no. 2, 2006, pp. 371–396., doi:10.1017/S1360674306001961. * Legate, Julie Anne. (2010). On how ''how'' is used instead of ''that''. ''Natural Language and Linguistic Theory'' 28:121-134. * Noonan, Michael. A grammar of Lango. Vol. 7. Walter de Gruyter, 2011. * Nordström, Jackie; Boye, Kasper (2016-07-11), "Complementizer semantics in the Germanic languages", ''Complementizer Semantics in European Languages'', De Gruyter, pp. 131–174, retrieved 2022-04-15 * Nyvad, Anne Mette; Christensen, Ken Ramshøj; Vikner, Sten (2017-01-01). "CP-recursion in Danish: A cP/CP-analysis". ''The Linguistic Review''. 34 (3). doi:10.1515/tlr-2017-0008.
ISSN An International Standard Serial Number (ISSN) is an eight-digit serial number used to uniquely identify a serial publication, such as a magazine. The ISSN is especially helpful in distinguishing between serials with the same title. ISSNs ...
 0167-6318. * * Sportiche, D., Koopman, H. J., & Stabler, E. P. (2014;2013;). ''An introduction to syntactic analysis and theory'' (1st;1; ed.). John Wiley & Sons Inc. {{lexical categories, state=collapsed Syntactic categories Parts of speech Grammatical marker type