Prototype theory is a theory of
categorization in
cognitive science, particularly in psychology and
cognitive linguistics
Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are con ...
, in which there is a graded degree of belonging to a conceptual category, and some members are more central than others. It emerged in 1971 with the work of psychologist
Eleanor Rosch
Eleanor Rosch (once known as Eleanor Rosch Heider;"Natural Categories", Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 4, No. 3, (May 1973), p. 328. born 1938) is an American psychologist. She is a professor of psychology at the University of California, Berkeley, s ...
, and it has been described as a "
Copernican revolution
The Copernican Revolution was the paradigm shift from the Ptolemaic model of the heavens, which described the cosmos as having Earth stationary at the center of the universe, to the heliocentric model with the Sun at the center of the Solar Sys ...
" in the theory of categorization for its departure from the traditional
Aristotelian categories.
[Coșeriu (2000)] It has been criticized by those that still endorse the traditional theory of categories, like linguist
Eugenio Coseriu and other proponents of the
structural semantics
Structural semantics (also structuralist semantics) is a linguistic school and paradigm that emerged in Europe from the 1930s, inspired by the structuralist linguistic movement started by Ferdinand de Saussure's 1916 work "'' Cours De Linguist ...
paradigm.
In this prototype theory, any given
concept
Concepts are defined as abstract ideas. They are understood to be the fundamental building blocks of the concept behind principles, thoughts and beliefs.
They play an important role in all aspects of cognition. As such, concepts are studied by ...
in any given language has a real world example that best represents this concept. For example: when asked to give an example of the concept ''
furniture'', a ''
couch'' is more frequently cited than, say, a ''
wardrobe
A wardrobe or armoire or almirah is a standing closet used for storing clothes. The earliest wardrobe was a chest, and it was not until some degree of luxury was attained in regal palaces and the castles of powerful nobles that separate accommo ...
''. Prototype theory has also been applied in
linguistics
Linguistics is the science, scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure ...
, as part of the mapping from
phonological structure to
semantics
Semantics (from grc, σημαντικός ''sēmantikós'', "significant") is the study of reference, meaning, or truth. The term can be used to refer to subfields of several distinct disciplines, including philosophy, linguistics and comp ...
.
In formulating prototype theory, Rosch drew in part from previous insights in particular the formulation of a category model based on
family resemblance
Family resemblance (german: Familienähnlichkeit, link=no) is a philosophical idea made popular by Ludwig Wittgenstein, with the best known exposition given in his posthumously published book ''Philosophical Investigations'' (1953). It argues tha ...
by
Wittgenstein
Ludwig Josef Johann Wittgenstein ( ; ; 26 April 1889 – 29 April 1951) was an Austrians, Austrian-British people, British philosopher who worked primarily in logic, the philosophy of mathematics, the philosophy of mind, and the philosophy o ...
(1953), and by
Roger Brown's ''How shall a thing be called?'' (1958).
[Croft and Cruse (2004) ''Cognitive Linguistics'' ch.4 pp.74-77]
Overview and terminology
The term ''prototype'', as defined in psychologist
Eleanor Rosch
Eleanor Rosch (once known as Eleanor Rosch Heider;"Natural Categories", Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 4, No. 3, (May 1973), p. 328. born 1938) is an American psychologist. She is a professor of psychology at the University of California, Berkeley, s ...
's study "Natural Categories", was initially defined as denoting a stimulus, which takes a salient position in the formation of a category, due to the fact that it is the first stimulus to be associated with that category. Rosch later defined it as the most central member of a category.
Rosch and others developed prototype theory as a response to, and radical departure from, the classical theory of concepts, which defines concepts by necessary and sufficient conditions. Necessary conditions refers to the set of features every instance of a concept must present, and sufficient conditions are those that no other entity possesses. Rather than defining concepts by features, the prototype theory defines categories based on either a specific artifact of that category or by a set of entities within the category that represent a prototypical member. The prototype of a category can be understood in lay terms by the object or member of a class most often associated with that class. The prototype is the center of the class, with all other members moving progressively further from the prototype, which leads to the gradation of categories. Every member of the class is not equally central in human cognition. As in the example of ''furniture'' above, ''couch'' is more central than ''wardrobe''. Contrary to the classical view, prototypes and gradations lead to an understanding of category membership not as an all-or-nothing approach, but as more of a web of interlocking categories which overlap.
In
Cognitive linguistics
Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are con ...
it has been argued that
linguistic categories also have a prototype structure, like categories of common words in a language.
John R Taylor
John is a common English name and surname:
* John (given name)
* John (surname)
John may also refer to:
New Testament
Works
* Gospel of John, a title often shortened to John
* First Epistle of John, often shortened to 1 John
* Second ...
(1995) ''Linguistic Categorization: Prototypes in Linguistic Theory'', 2nd ed., ch.2 p.21
Categories
Basic level categories
The other notion related to prototypes is that of a ''basic level'' in cognitive categorization. Basic categories are relatively homogeneous in terms of sensory-motor
affordance
Affordance is what the environment offers the individual. American psychologist James J. Gibson coined the term in his 1966 book, ''The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems'', and it occurs in many of his earlier essays. However, his best-know ...
s — a chair is associated with bending of one's knees, a fruit with picking it up and putting it in your mouth, etc. At the subordinate level (e.g.
entist's chairs itchen chairsetc.) few significant features can be added to that of the basic level; whereas at the superordinate level, these conceptual similarities are hard to pinpoint. A picture of a chair is easy to draw (or visualize), but drawing furniture would be more difficult.
Linguist Eleanor Rosch defines the basic level as that level that has the highest degree of cue validity. Thus, a category like
nimal Nimal may refer to
*Nimal Bandara, Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Gamini Amaratunga, Sri Lankan judge
*Nimal Gunaratne, Sri Lankan air force officer
*Nimal Mendis, Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Piyatissa (born 1968), Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Raja ...
may have a prototypical member, but no cognitive visual representation. On the other hand, basic categories in
nimal Nimal may refer to
*Nimal Bandara, Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Gamini Amaratunga, Sri Lankan judge
*Nimal Gunaratne, Sri Lankan air force officer
*Nimal Mendis, Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Piyatissa (born 1968), Sri Lankan politician
*Nimal Raja ...
i.e.
og ird ish
Ish may refer to:
*Ish (name) also ancient Hebrew word for Man at Genesis 2:23, also Ish-shah for Woman
*Chazon Ish, sobriquet of Rabbi Avraham Yeshayahu Karelitz
*the Sanskrit for "lord", see Ishvara
* ''...ish'' (audio drama), Doctor Who audio d ...
are full of informational content and can easily be categorized in terms of
Gestalt
Gestalt may refer to:
Psychology
* Gestalt psychology, a school of psychology
* Gestalt therapy, a form of psychotherapy
* Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test, an assessment of development disorders
* Gestalt Practice, a practice of self-exploration ...
and semantic features.
Clearly semantic models based on attribute-value pairs fail to identify privileged levels in the hierarchy. Functionally, it is thought that basic level categories are a decomposition of the world into maximally
informative categories. Thus, they
* maximize the number of attributes shared by members of the category, and
* minimize the number of attributes shared with other categories
However, the notion of Basic Level is problematic, e.g. whereas dog as a basic category is a species, bird or fish are at a higher level, etc. Similarly, the notion of frequency is very closely tied to the basic level, but is hard to pinpoint.
More problems arise when the notion of a prototype is applied to lexical categories other than the noun. Verbs, for example, seem to defy a clear prototype:
o runis hard to split up in more or less central members.
In her 1975 paper, Rosch asked 200 American college students to rate, on a scale of 1 to 7, whether they regarded certain items as good examples of the category ''furniture''. These items ranged from chair and sofa, ranked number 1, to a love seat (number 10), to a lamp (number 31), all the way to a telephone, ranked number 60.
While one may differ from this list in terms of cultural specifics, the point is that such a graded categorization is likely to be present in all cultures. Further evidence that some members of a category are more privileged than others came from experiments involving:
: 1. ''Response Times'': in which queries involving prototypical members (e.g. ''is a robin a bird'') elicited faster response times than for non-prototypical members.
: 2. ''Priming'': When primed with the higher-level (superordinate) category, subjects were faster in identifying if two words are the same. Thus, after flashing ''furniture'', the equivalence of ''chair-chair'' is detected more rapidly than ''stove-stove''.
: 3. ''Exemplars'': When asked to name a few exemplars, the more prototypical items came up more frequently.
Subsequent to Rosch's work, prototype effects have been investigated widely in areas such as colour cognition, and also for more abstract notions: subjects may be asked, e.g. "to what degree is this narrative an instance of telling a lie?". Similar work has been done on actions (verbs like look, kill, speak, walk
ulman:83, adjectives like "tall", etc.
Another aspect in which Prototype Theory departs from traditional
Aristotelian categorization
The ''Categories'' (Greek Κατηγορίαι ''Katēgoriai''; Latin ''Categoriae'' or ''Praedicamenta'') is a text from Aristotle's ''Organon'' that enumerates all the possible kinds of things that can be the subject or the predicate of a prop ...
is that there do not appear to be
natural kind
"Natural kind" is an intellectual grouping, or categorizing of things, in a manner that is reflective of the actual world and not just human interests. Some treat it as a classification identifying some structure of truth and reality that exists wh ...
categories (bird, dog) vs. artifacts (toys, vehicles).
A common comparison is the use of prototype or the use of exemplars in category classification. Medin, Altom, and Murphy found that using a mixture of prototype and exemplar information, participants were more accurately able to judge categories. Participants who were presented with prototype values classified based on similarity to stored prototypes and stored exemplars, whereas participants who only had experience with exemplar only relied on the similarity to stored exemplars. Smith and Minda looked at the use of prototypes and exemplars in dot-pattern category learning. They found that participants used more prototypes than they used exemplars, with the prototypes being the center of the category, and exemplars surrounding it.
Distance between concepts
The notion of prototypes is related to
Wittgenstein
Ludwig Josef Johann Wittgenstein ( ; ; 26 April 1889 – 29 April 1951) was an Austrians, Austrian-British people, British philosopher who worked primarily in logic, the philosophy of mathematics, the philosophy of mind, and the philosophy o ...
's (later) discomfort with the traditional notion of category. This influential theory has resulted in a view of semantic components more as ''possible'' rather than necessary contributors to the meaning of texts. His discussion on the category ''game'' is particularly incisive:
Consider for example the proceedings that we call 'games'. I mean board games, card games, ball games, Olympic games, and so on. What is common to them all? Don't say, "There must be something common, or they would not be called 'games'"--but look and see whether there is anything common to all. For if you look at them you will not see something common to all, but similarities, relationships, and a whole series of them at that. To repeat: don't think, but look! Look for example at board games, with their multifarious relationships. Now pass to card games; here you find many correspondences with the first group, but many common features drop out, and others appear. When we pass next to ball games, much that is common is retained, but much is lost. Are they all 'amusing'? Compare chess with noughts and crosses. Or is there always winning and losing, or competition between players? Think of patience. In ball games there is winning and losing; but when a child throws his ball at the wall and catches it again, this feature has disappeared. Look at the parts played by skill and luck; and at the difference between skill in chess and skill in tennis. Think now of games like ring-a-ring-a-roses; here is the element of amusement, but how many other characteristic features have disappeared! And we can go through the many, many other groups of games in the same way; can see how similarities crop up and disappear. And the result of this examination is: we see a complicated network of similarities overlapping and criss-crossing: sometimes overall similarities, sometimes similarities of detail.
Wittgenstein's theory of
family resemblance
Family resemblance (german: Familienähnlichkeit, link=no) is a philosophical idea made popular by Ludwig Wittgenstein, with the best known exposition given in his posthumously published book ''Philosophical Investigations'' (1953). It argues tha ...
describes the phenomenon when people group concepts based on a series of overlapping features, rather than by one feature which exists throughout all members of the category. For example, basketball and baseball share the use of a ball, and baseball and chess share the feature of a winner, etc, rather than one defining feature of "games". Therefore, there is a distance between focal, or prototypical members of the category, and those that continue outwards from them, linked by shared features.
Recently,
Peter Gärdenfors
Björn ''Peter'' Gärdenfors (born 21 September 1949) is professor of cognitive science at the University of Lund, Sweden.
Gärdenfors is a recipient of the Gad Rausing Prize (Swedish: ''Rausingpriset''). He received his doctorate from Lund Univ ...
has elaborated a possible partial explanation of prototype theory in terms of multi-dimensional feature spaces called
conceptual space A conceptual space is a geometric structure that represents a number of quality dimensions, which denote basic features by which concepts and objects can be compared, such as weight, color, taste, temperature, pitch, and the three ordinary spatial ...
s, where a category is defined in terms of a conceptual distance. More central members of a category are "between" the peripheral members. He postulates that most ''natural'' categories exhibit a convexity in conceptual space, in that if x and y are elements of a category, and if z is ''between'' x and y, then z is also likely to belong to the category.
Combining categories
Within language we find instances of combined categories, such as ''tall man'' or ''small elephant''. Combining categories was a problem for
extensional semantics, where the semantics of a word such as ''red'' is to be defined as the set of objects having this property. This does not apply as well to modifiers such as ''small''; a ''small mouse'' is very different from a ''small elephant''.
These combinations pose a lesser problem in terms of prototype theory. In situations involving adjectives (e.g. ''tall''), one encounters the question of whether or not the prototype of
all
All or ALL may refer to:
Language
* All, an indefinite pronoun in English
* All, one of the English determiners
* Allar language (ISO 639-3 code)
* Allative case (abbreviated ALL)
Music
* All (band), an American punk rock band
* ''All'' (All al ...
is a 6 foot tall man, or a 400-foot skyscraper. The solution emerges by contextualizing the notion of prototype in terms of the object being modified. This extends even more radically in compounds such as ''red wine'' or ''red hair'' which are hardly ''red'' in the prototypical sense, but the red indicates merely a shift from the prototypical colour of wine or hair respectively. The addition of ''red'' shifts the prototype from the one of ''hair'' to that of ''red hair.'' The prototype is changed by additional specific information, and combines features from the prototype of ''red'' and ''wine''.
Dynamic structure and distance
Mikulincer, Mario & Paz, Dov & Kedem, Perry focused on the dynamic nature of prototypes and how represented semantic categories actually changes due to emotional states. The 4 part study assessed the relationships between situational stress and trait anxiety and the way people organize the hierarchical level at which semantic stimuli are categorized, the way people categorize natural objects, the narrowing of the breadth of categories and the proneness to use less inclusive levels of categorization instead of more inclusive ones.
Critique
Prototype theory has been criticized by those that still endorse the
classic theory of categories, like linguist
Eugenio Coseriu and other proponents of the
structural semantics
Structural semantics (also structuralist semantics) is a linguistic school and paradigm that emerged in Europe from the 1930s, inspired by the structuralist linguistic movement started by Ferdinand de Saussure's 1916 work "'' Cours De Linguist ...
paradigm.
Exemplar theory
Douglas L. Medin and Marguerite M. Schaffer showed by experiment that a context theory of classification which derives concepts purely from exemplars (cf.
exemplar theory) worked better than a class of theories that included prototype theory.
Graded categorization
Linguists, including
Stephen Laurence writing with
Eric Margolis, have suggested problems with the prototype theory. In their 1999 paper, they raise several issues. One of which is that prototype theory does not intrinsically guarantee graded categorization. When subjects were asked to rank how well certain members exemplify the category, they rated some members above others. For example robins were seen as being "birdier" than ostriches, but when asked whether these categories are "all-or-nothing" or have fuzzier boundaries, the subjects stated that they were defined, "all-or-nothing" categories. Laurence and Margolis concluded that "prototype structure has no implication for whether subjects represent a category as being graded" (p. 33).
Compound concepts
Daniel Osherson and Edward Smith raised the issue of ''pet fish'' for which the prototype might be a
guppy
The guppy (), also known as millionfish and rainbow fish, is one of the world's most widely distributed tropical fish and one of the most popular freshwater aquarium fish species. It is a member of the family Poeciliidae and, like almost all ...
kept in a bowl in someone's house. The prototype for ''pet'' might be a dog or cat, and the prototype for ''fish'' might be trout or salmon. However, the features of these prototypes do not present in the prototype for ''pet fish'', therefore this prototype must be generated from something other than its constituent parts.
Antonio Lieto and Gian Luca Pozzato
have proposed a typicality-based compositional logic (TCL) that is able to account for both complex human-like concept combinations (like the PET-FISH problem) and conceptual blending. Thus, their framework shows how concepts expressed as prototypes can account for the phenomenon of prototypical compositionality in concept combination.
See also
*
Composite photography
*
Composite portrait
Composite portraiture (also known as composite photographs) is a technique invented by Sir Francis Galton in the 1880s after a suggestion by Herbert Spencer for registering photographs of human faces on the two eyes to create an "average" photogra ...
*
Exemplar theory
*
Family resemblance
Family resemblance (german: Familienähnlichkeit, link=no) is a philosophical idea made popular by Ludwig Wittgenstein, with the best known exposition given in his posthumously published book ''Philosophical Investigations'' (1953). It argues tha ...
*
Folksonomy
Folksonomy is a classification system in which end users apply public tags to online items, typically to make those items easier for themselves or others to find later. Over time, this can give rise to a classification system based on those tags ...
*
Frame semantics
*
Platonic ideal
The theory of Forms or theory of Ideas is a philosophical theory, fuzzy concept, or world-view, attributed to Plato, that the physical world is not as real or true as timeless, absolute, unchangeable ideas. According to this theory, ideas in th ...
*
Semantic feature-comparison model The semantic feature comparison model is used "to derive predictions about categorization times in a situation where a subject must rapidly decide whether a test item is a member of a particular target category".Smith, E. E., Shoben. E. J., and Rips ...
*
Similarity (philosophy)
In philosophy, similarity or resemblance is a relation between objects that constitutes how much these objects are alike. Similarity comes in degrees: e.g. oranges are more similar to apples than to the moon. It is traditionally seen as an interna ...
*
Intuitive statistics Intuitive statistics, or folk statistics, refers to the cognitive phenomenon where organisms use data to make generalizations and predictions about the world. This can be a small amount of sample data or training instances, which in turn contribute ...
Footnotes
References
*
Berlin, B. &
Kay, P. (1969): ''
Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution'', Berkeley.
*
Coseriu, E., Willems, K and Leuschner, T, (2000)
Structural Semantics and 'Cognitive' Semantics', in Logos and Language
* Dirven, R. & Taylor, J. R. (1988): "The conceptualisation of vertical Space in English: The Case of Tall", in: Rudzka-Ostyn, B.(ed): ''Topics in Cognitive Linguistics''. Amsterdam.
Galton, F. (1878). Composite portraits. ''Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland'', Vol.8, pp.132–142.doi=10.2307/2841021
* Gatsgeb, H. Z., Dundas, E. M., Minshew, M. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2012). Category formation in autism: Can individuals with autism form categories and prototypes of dot patterns?. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 42(8), 1694-1704.
* Gatsgeb, H. Z., Wilkinson, D. A., Minshew, M. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2011)
Can individuals with autism abstract prototypes of natural faces? Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 41(12), 1609-1618. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1190-4
*
* Lakoff, G. (1987): ''Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind'', London.
* Lieto, A., Pozzato, G.L. (2019):
A description logic framework for commonsense conceptual combination integrating typicality, probabilities and cognitive heuristics', Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, doi: 10.1080/0952813X.2019.1672799.
*
Loftus, E.F.,
Spreading Activation Within Semantic Categories: Comments on Rosch’s “Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), p. 234-240.
* Medin, D. L., Altom, M. W., & Murphy, T. D. (1984)
Given versus induced category representations: Use of prototype and exemplar information in classification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(3), 333-352. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.10.3.333
* Mario Mikulincer Peri Kedem & Dov Paz (1990) The impact of trait anxiety and situational stress on the categorization of natural objects, Anxiety Research, 2:2, 85-101, DOI: 10.1080/08917779008249328
* Mikulincer, Mario & Kedem-Friedrich, Peri & Paz, Dov. (1990). Anxiety and categorization—1. The structure and boundaries of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences. 11. 805-814. 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90189-X.
* Mikulincer, Mario & Paz, Dov & Kedem-Friedrich, Peri. (1990). Anxiety and categorization—2. Hierarchical levels of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences. 11. 815-821. 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90190-3.
* Molesworth, C. J., Bowler, D. M., & Hampton, J. A. (2005)
Extracting prototypes from exemplars what can corpus data tell us about concept representation? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(6), 661-672. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00383.x
* Molesworth, C. J., Bowler, D. M., & Hampton, J. A. (2008)
When prototypes are not best: Judgments made by children with autism Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 38(9), 1721-1730. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0557-7
*
Rosch, E., "Classification of Real-World Objects: Origins and Representations in Cognition", pp. 212–222 in Johnson-Laird, P.N. & Wason, P.C., ''Thinking: Readings in Cognitive Science'', Cambridge University Press, (Cambridge), 1977.
*
Rosch, E. (1975): “Cognitive Reference Points”, ''Cognitive Psychology'' 7, 532-547.
*
Rosch, E., "Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories", ''Journal of Experimental Psychology: General'', Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), pp. 192–233.
*
Rosch, E.H. (1973): "Natural categories", ''Cognitive Psychology'' 4, 328-350.
*
Rosch, E., "Principles of Categorization", pp. 27–48 in Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B.B. (eds), ''Cognition and Categorization'', Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, (Hillsdale), 1978.
*
Rosch, E., "Prototype Classification and Logical Classification: The Two Systems", pp. 73–86 in Scholnick, E.K. (ed), ''New Trends in Conceptual Representation: Challenges to Piaget’s Theory?'', Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, 1983.
*
Rosch, E., "Reclaiming Concepts", ''Journal of Consciousness Studies'', Vol.6, Nos.11-12, (November/December 1999), pp. 61–77.
*
Rosch, E., "Reply to Loftus", ''Journal of Experimental Psychology: General'', Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), pp. 241–243.
*
Rosch, E. & Mervis, C.B., "Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories", ''Cognitive Psychology'', Vol.7, No.4, (October 1975), pp. 573–605.
*
Rosch, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P., ''Basic Objects in Natural Categories, Working Paper No.43'', Language Behaviour Research Laboratory, University of California (Berkeley), 1975.
*
Rosch, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P., "Basic Objects in Natural Categories", ''Cognitive Psychology'', Vol.8, No.3, (July 1976), pp. 382–439.
* Smith, J. D., & Minda, J. P. (2002)
Distinguishing prototype-based and exemplar-based processes in dot-pattern category learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 1433-1458. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1433
* Taylor, J. R.(2003): ''Linguistic Categorization'', Oxford University Press.
*
Wittgenstein, L., ''
Philosophical Investigations
''Philosophical Investigations'' (german: Philosophische Untersuchungen) is a work by the philosopher Ludwig Wittgenstein, published posthumously in 1953.
''Philosophical Investigations'' is divided into two parts, consisting of what Wittgens ...
(Philosophische Untersuchungen)'', Blackwell Publishers, 2001 ().
{{DEFAULTSORT:Prototype Theory
Cognitive science
Semantics
Psychological theories
Semantic relations
Cognitive linguistics
Philosophy of language