The model of hierarchical complexity (MHC) is a framework for scoring how complex a
behavior
Behavior (American English) or behaviour (British English) is the range of actions of Individual, individuals, organisms, systems or Artificial intelligence, artificial entities in some environment. These systems can include other systems or or ...
is, such as
verbal reasoning or other
cognitive
Cognition is the "mental action or process of acquiring knowledge and understanding through thought, experience, and the senses". It encompasses all aspects of intellectual functions and processes such as: perception, attention, thought, ...
tasks. It quantifies the order of
hierarchical
A hierarchy (from Greek: , from , 'president of sacred rites') is an arrangement of items (objects, names, values, categories, etc.) that are represented as being "above", "below", or "at the same level as" one another. Hierarchy is an importan ...
complexity of a task based on mathematical principles of how the information is organized, in terms of
information science.
[ Commons, 2007] This model was developed by
Michael Commons
Michael Lamport Commons (born 1939) is a theoretical behavioral scientist and a complex systems scientist. He developed the model of hierarchical complexity.
Life and work
Michael Lamport Commons was born in 1939 in Los Angeles and grew up in ...
and Francis Richards in the early 1980s.
Overview
The model of hierarchical complexity (MHC) is a
formal theory and a
mathematical psychology
Mathematical psychology is an approach to psychology, psychological research that is based on mathematical modeling of perceptual, thought, Cognition, cognitive and motor processes, and on the establishment of law-like rules that relate quantifi ...
framework for scoring how complex a behavior is.
[ Commons & Pekker, 2008; Commons et al., 2014] Developed by
Michael Lamport Commons and colleagues,
[ Commons et al., 1998] it quantifies the order of hierarchical complexity of a task based on mathematical principles of how the information is organized, in terms of
information science.
[ Commons & Richards, 1984a, Commons & Richards, 1984b] Its forerunner was the general stage model.
Behaviors that may be scored include those of individual humans or their social groupings (e.g., organizations, governments, societies), animals, or machines. It enables scoring the hierarchical complexity of task accomplishment in any domain. It is based on the very simple notions that higher order task actions:
#are defined in terms of the next lower ones (creating hierarchy);
#organize the next lower actions;
#organize lower actions in a non-arbitrary way (differentiating them from simple chains of behavior).
It is
cross-cultural
Cross-cultural may refer to:
*cross-cultural studies, a comparative tendency in various fields of cultural analysis
*cross-cultural communication, a field of study that looks at how people from differing culture, cultural backgrounds communicate
* ...
ly and cross-species valid. The reason it applies cross-culturally is that the scoring is based on the mathematical complexity of the hierarchical organization of information. Scoring does not depend upon the content of the information (e.g., what is done, said, written, or analyzed) but upon how the information is organized.
The MHC is a non-
mentalistic model of
developmental stages.
It specifies 16 orders of hierarchical complexity and their corresponding
stages. It is different from previous proposals about developmental stage applied to humans; instead of attributing behavioral changes across a person's age to the development of mental structures or
schema
Schema may refer to:
Science and technology
* SCHEMA (bioinformatics), an algorithm used in protein engineering
* Schema (genetic algorithms), a set of programs or bit strings that have some genotypic similarity
* Schema.org, a web markup vocab ...
, this model posits that task sequences of task behaviors form hierarchies that become increasingly complex. Because less complex tasks must be completed and practiced before more complex tasks can be acquired, this accounts for the developmental changes seen in an individual persons' performance of complex tasks. For example, a person cannot perform
arithmetic
Arithmetic is an elementary branch of mathematics that deals with numerical operations like addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. In a wider sense, it also includes exponentiation, extraction of roots, and taking logarithms.
...
until the numeral representations of numbers are learned, or a person cannot operationally multiply the sums of numbers until addition is learned. However, as much as natural intelligence helps human to understand some numbers, it does not play a complete role in multiplying large numbers without learning additions.
The creators of the MHC claim that previous theories of stage have confounded the stimulus and response in assessing stage by simply scoring responses and ignoring the task or stimulus.
The MHC separates the task or stimulus from the performance. The participant's performance on a task of a given complexity represents the stage of developmental complexity.
Previous stage theories were unsatisfying to Commons and Richards because the theories did not show the existence of the stages more than describing sequential changes in human behavior. This led them to create a list of two concepts they felt a successful developmental theory should address. The two ideas they wanted to study were (1) the hierarchical complexity of the task to be solved and (2) the psychology, sociology, and anthropology of the task performance (and the development of the performance).
Vertical complexity of tasks performed
One major basis for this developmental theory is
task analysis
Task analysis is a fundamental tool of human factors engineering. It entails analyzing how a task is accomplished, including a detailed description of both manual and mental activities, task and element durations, task frequency, task allocation, ...
. The study of ideal tasks, including their instantiation in the real world, has been the basis of the branch of stimulus control called
psychophysics
Psychophysics is the field of psychology which quantitatively investigates the relationship between physical stimulus (physiology), stimuli and the sensation (psychology), sensations and perceptions they produce. Psychophysics has been described ...
. Tasks are defined as sequences of contingencies, each presenting stimuli and each requiring a behavior or a sequence of behaviors that must occur in some non-arbitrary fashion. The complexity of behaviors necessary to complete a task can be specified using the horizontal complexity and vertical complexity definitions described below. Behavior is examined with respect to the analytically-known complexity of the task.
Tasks are
quantal in nature. They are either completed correctly or not completed at all. There is no intermediate state (''
tertium non datur''). For this reason, the model characterizes all stages as
P-hard and functionally distinct. The orders of hierarchical complexity are
quantized like the
electron
The electron (, or in nuclear reactions) is a subatomic particle with a negative one elementary charge, elementary electric charge. It is a fundamental particle that comprises the ordinary matter that makes up the universe, along with up qua ...
atomic orbitals
In quantum mechanics, an atomic orbital () is a function describing the location and wave-like behavior of an electron in an atom. This function describes an electron's charge distribution around the atom's nucleus, and can be used to calc ...
around the
nucleus: each task difficulty has an order of hierarchical complexity required to complete it correctly, analogous to the atomic
Slater determinant
In quantum mechanics, a Slater determinant is an expression that describes the wave function of a multi-fermionic system. It satisfies anti-symmetry requirements, and consequently the Pauli principle, by changing sign upon exchange of two fermion ...
. Since tasks of a given quantified order of hierarchical complexity require actions of a given order of hierarchical complexity to perform them, the stage of the participant's task performance is equivalent to the order of complexity of the successfully completed task. The quantal feature of tasks is thus particularly instrumental in stage assessment because the scores obtained for stages are likewise discrete.
Every task contains a multitude of subtasks. When the subtasks are carried out by the participant in a required order, the task in question is successfully completed. Therefore, the model asserts that all tasks fit in some configured sequence of tasks, making it possible to precisely determine the hierarchical order of task complexity. Tasks vary in complexity in two ways: either as ''horizontal'' (involving classical information); or as ''vertical'' (involving hierarchical information).
Horizontal complexity
Classical information describes the number of "yes–no" questions it takes to do a task. For example, if one asked a person across the room whether a penny came up heads when they flipped it, their saying "heads" would transmit 1 bit of "horizontal" information. If there were 2 pennies, one would have to ask at least two questions, one about each penny. Hence, each additional 1-bit question would add another bit. Let us say they had a four-faced top with the faces numbered 1, 2, 3, and 4. Instead of spinning it, they tossed it against a backboard as one does with dice in a game of
craps
Craps is a dice game in which players gambling, bet on the outcomes of the roll of a pair of dice. Players can wager money against each other (playing "street craps") or against a bank ("casino craps"). Because it requires little equipment, " ...
. Again, there would be 2 bits. One could ask them whether the face had an even number. If it did, one would then ask if it were a 2. Horizontal complexity, then, is the sum of bits required by just such tasks as these.
Vertical complexity
Hierarchical complexity refers to the number of
recursion
Recursion occurs when the definition of a concept or process depends on a simpler or previous version of itself. Recursion is used in a variety of disciplines ranging from linguistics to logic. The most common application of recursion is in m ...
s that the coordinating actions must perform on a set of primary elements. Actions at a higher order of hierarchical complexity: (a) are defined in terms of actions at the next lower order of hierarchical complexity; (b) organize and transform the lower-order actions (see Figure 2); (c) produce organizations of lower-order actions that are qualitatively new and not arbitrary, and cannot be accomplished by those lower-order actions alone. Once these conditions have been met, we say the higher-order action coordinates the actions of the next lower order.
To illustrate how lower actions get organized into more hierarchically complex actions, let us turn to a simple example. Completing the entire operation 3 × (4 + 1) constitutes a task requiring the
distributive act. That act non-
arbitrarily orders adding and multiplying to coordinate them. The distributive act is therefore one order more hierarchically complex than the acts of adding and multiplying alone; it indicates the singular proper sequence of the simpler actions. Although simply adding results in the same answer, people who can do both display a greater freedom of mental functioning. Additional layers of abstraction can be applied. Thus, the order of complexity of the task is determined through analyzing the demands of each task by breaking it down into its constituent parts.
The hierarchical complexity of a task refers to the number of
concatenation
In formal language theory and computer programming, string concatenation is the operation of joining character strings end-to-end. For example, the concatenation of "snow" and "ball" is "snowball". In certain formalizations of concatenati ...
operations it contains, that is, the number of recursions that the coordinating actions must perform. An order-three task has three concatenation operations. A task of order three operates on one or more tasks of vertical order two and a task of order two operates on one or more tasks of vertical order one (the simplest tasks).
Stages of development
Stage theories describe
human
Humans (''Homo sapiens'') or modern humans are the most common and widespread species of primate, and the last surviving species of the genus ''Homo''. They are Hominidae, great apes characterized by their Prehistory of nakedness and clothing ...
organism
An organism is any life, living thing that functions as an individual. Such a definition raises more problems than it solves, not least because the concept of an individual is also difficult. Many criteria, few of them widely accepted, have be ...
ic and/or
technological
Technology is the application of conceptual knowledge to achieve practical goals, especially in a reproducible way. The word ''technology'' can also mean the products resulting from such efforts, including both tangible tools such as ute ...
evolution as systems that move through a pattern of distinct stages over time. Here development is described formally in terms of the model of hierarchical complexity (MHC).
Formal definition of stage
Since actions are defined inductively, so is the function ''h'', known as the order of the hierarchical complexity. To each action ''A'', we wish to associate a notion of that action's hierarchical complexity, ''h(A)''. Given a collection of actions A and a participant ''S'' performing A, the ''stage of performance'' of ''S'' on A is the highest order of the actions in A completed successfully at least once, i.e., it is: stage (''S'', A) = max. Thus, the notion of stage is
discontinuous, having the same transitional gaps as the orders of hierarchical complexity. This is in accordance with previous definitions.
Because MHC stages are conceptualized in terms of the hierarchical complexity of tasks rather than in terms of mental representations (as in Piaget's stages), the highest stage represents successful performances on the most hierarchically complex tasks rather than intellectual maturity.
Stages of hierarchical complexity
The following table gives descriptions of each stage in the MHC.
Relationship with Piaget's theory
The MHC builds on
Piagetian theory but differs from it in many ways; notably the MHC has additional higher stages. In both theories, one finds:
- Higher-order actions defined in terms of lower-order actions. This forces the hierarchical nature of the relations and makes the higher-order tasks include the lower ones and requires that lower-order actions are hierarchically contained within the relative definitions of the higher-order tasks.
- Higher-order of complexity actions organize those lower-order actions. This makes them more powerful. Lower-order actions are organized by the actions with a higher order of complexity, i.e., the more complex tasks.
What
Commons et al. (1998) have added includes:
- Higher-order-of-complexity actions organize those lower-order actions in a non-arbitrary way.
This makes it possible for the model's application to meet real world requirements, including the empirical and analytic. Arbitrary organization of lower order of complexity actions, possible in the Piagetian theory, despite the hierarchical definition structure, leaves the functional correlates of the interrelationships of tasks of differential complexity formulations ill-defined.
Moreover, the model is consistent with the
neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development
Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development criticize and build upon Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development.
Overview
The neo-Piagetian theories aim to correct one or more of the following weaknesses in Piaget's theory:
* Piaget's de ...
. According to these theories, progression to higher stages or levels of cognitive development is caused by increases in processing efficiency and
working memory
Working memory is a cognitive system with a limited capacity that can Memory, hold information temporarily. It is important for reasoning and the guidance of decision-making and behavior. Working memory is often used synonymously with short-term m ...
capacity. That is, higher-order stages place increasingly higher demands on these functions of information processing, so that their order of appearance reflects the information processing possibilities at successive ages.
The following dimensions are inherent in the application:
# Task and performance are separated.
# All tasks have an order of hierarchical complexity.
# There is only one sequence of orders of hierarchical complexity.
# Hence, there is structure of the whole for
ideal tasks and actions.
# There are transitional gaps between the orders of hierarchical complexity.
# Stage is defined as the most hierarchically complex task solved.
# There are discrete gaps in
Rasch scaled stage of performance.
# Performance stage is different task area to task area.
# There is no structure of the whole—
horizontal décalage—for performance. It is not inconsistency in thinking within a developmental stage. Décalage is the normal modal state of affairs.
Orders and corresponding stages
The MHC specifies 16 orders of hierarchical complexity and their corresponding stages, positing that each of Piaget's substages, in fact, are robustly hard stages.
[ Commons, Crone-Todd, & Chen, 2014] The MHC adds five postformal stages to Piaget's developmental trajectory: systematic stage 12, metasystematic stage 13, paradigmatic stage 14, cross-paradigmatic stage 15, and meta-cross-paradigmatic stage 16. It may be the Piaget's ''consolidate'' formal stage is the same as the ''systematic'' stage. The sequence is as follows: (0) calculatory, (1) automatic, (2) sensory & motor, (3) circular sensory-motor, (4) sensory-motor, (5) nominal, (6) sentential, (7) preoperational, (8) primary, (9) concrete, (10) abstract, (11) formal, and the five postformal: (12) systematic, (13) metasystematic, (14) paradigmatic, (15) cross-paradigmatic, and (16) meta-cross-paradigmatic. The first four stages (0–3) correspond to Piaget's sensorimotor stage at which infants and very young children perform. Adolescents and adults can perform at any of the subsequent stages. MHC stages 4 through 5 correspond to Piaget's pre-operational stage; 6 through 8 correspond to his concrete operational stage; and 9 through 11 correspond to his formal operational stage.
More complex behaviors characterize multiple system models. The four highest stages in the MHC are not represented in Piaget's model. The higher stages of the MHC have extensively influenced the field of
positive adult development. Some adults are said to develop alternatives to, and perspectives on, formal operations; they use formal operations within a "higher" system of operations. Some theorists call the more complex orders of cognitive tasks "
postformal thought", but other theorists argue that these higher orders cannot exactly be labelled as postformal thought.
Jordan (2018) argued that unidimensional models such as the MHC, which measure level of complexity of some behavior, refer to only one of many aspects of adult development, and that other variables are needed (in addition to unidimensional measures of complexity) for a fuller description of adult development.
Empirical research using the model
The MHC has a broad range of applicability.
Its mathematical foundation permits it to be used by anyone examining task performance that is organized into stages. It is designed to assess development based on the order of complexity which the actor utilizes to organize information. The model thus allows for a standard
quantitative
Quantitative may refer to:
* Quantitative research, scientific investigation of quantitative properties
* Quantitative analysis (disambiguation)
* Quantitative verse, a metrical system in poetry
* Statistics, also known as quantitative analysis
...
analysis of developmental complexity in any cultural setting. Other advantages of this model include its avoidance of
mentalistic explanations, as well as its use of quantitative principles which are universally applicable in any context.
The following practitioners can use the MHC to quantitatively assess developmental stages:
*
Cross-cultural
Cross-cultural may refer to:
*cross-cultural studies, a comparative tendency in various fields of cultural analysis
*cross-cultural communication, a field of study that looks at how people from differing culture, cultural backgrounds communicate
* ...
developmentalists
*
Animal developmentalists
*
Evolutionary psychologists
*
Organizational psychologists
* Developmental
political psychologists
*
Learning theorists
*
Perception
Perception () is the organization, identification, and interpretation of sensory information in order to represent and understand the presented information or environment. All perception involves signals that go through the nervous syste ...
researchers
*
Historians of science
The history of science covers the development of science from ancient times to the present. It encompasses all three major branches of science: natural, social, and formal. Protoscience, early sciences, and natural philosophies such as al ...
*
Educator
A teacher, also called a schoolteacher or formally an educator, is a person who helps students to acquire knowledge, competence, or virtue, via the practice of teaching.
''Informally'' the role of teacher may be taken on by anyone (e.g. w ...
s
*
Therapist
A therapist is a person who offers any kinds of therapy. Therapists are trained professionals in the field of any types of services like psychologists, social workers, counselors, etc. They are helpful in counseling individuals for various mental ...
s
*
Anthropologist
An anthropologist is a scientist engaged in the practice of anthropology. Anthropologists study aspects of humans within past and present societies. Social anthropology, cultural anthropology and philosophical anthropology study the norms, values ...
s
List of examples
In one representative study,
Commons, Goodheart, and Dawson (1997) found, using
Rasch analysis (
Rasch, 1980), that hierarchical complexity of a given task predicts stage of a performance, the correlation being r = 0.92. Correlations of similar magnitude have been found in a number of the studies. The following are examples of tasks studied using the model of hierarchical complexity or
Kurt W. Fischer's similar skill theory:
* Algebra (
Commons, Giri, & Harrigan, 2014)
* Animal stages (
Commons & Miller, 2004)
* Atheism (
Commons-Miller, 2005)
* Attachment and loss (
Commons, 1991;
Miller & Lee, 2000)
* Balance beam and pendulum (
Commons, Goodheart, & Bresette, 1995;
Commons, Giri, & Harrigan, 2014)
* Contingencies of reinforcement (
Commons & Giri, 2016)
* Counselor stages (
Lovell, 2002)
* Empathy of hominids (
Commons & Wolfsont, 2002)
* Epistemology (
Kitchener & Fischer, 1990;
Kitchener & King, 1990)
* Evaluative reasoning (
Dawson, 2000)
* Four story problem (
Commons, Richards & Kuhn, 1982;
Kallio & Helkama, 1991)
* Good education (
Dawson-Tunik, 2004)
* Good interpersonal relations (
Armon, 1984a;
Armon, 1984b;
Armon, 1989)
* Good work (
Armon, 1993)
* Honesty and kindness (
Lamborn, Fischer & Pipp, 1994)
* Informed consent (
Commons & Rodriguez, 1990;
Commons & Rodriguez, 1993;
Commons, Goodheart, Rodriguez, & Gutheil, 2006)
* Language stages (
Commons et al., 2007)
* Leadership before and after crises (
Oliver, 2004)
* Loevinger's sentence completion task (
Cook-Greuter, 1990)
* Moral judgment (
Armon & Dawson, 1997;
Dawson, 2000)
* Music (Beethoven) (
Funk, 1989)
* Physics tasks (
Inhelder & Piaget, 1958)
* Political development (
Sonnert & Commons, 1994)
* Report patient's prior crimes (
Commons, Lee, Gutheil, et al., 1995)
* Social perspective-taking (
Commons & Rodriguez, 1990;
Commons & Rodriguez, 1993)
* Spirituality (
Miller & Cook-Greuter, 1994)
* Tool making of hominids (
Commons & Miller 2002)
* Views of the good life (
Armon, 1984b;
Danaher, 1993;
Dawson, 2000;
Lam, 1995)
* Workplace culture (
Commons, Krause, Fayer, & Meaney, 1993)
* Workplace organization (
Bowman, 1996)
As of 2014, people and institutes from all the major continents of the world, except Africa, have used the model of hierarchical complexity.
[ Commons & Chen, 2014] Because the model is very simple and is based on analysis of tasks and not just performances, it is dynamic.
With the help of the model, it is possible to quantify the occurrence and progression of transition processes in task performances at any order of hierarchical complexity.
Criticisms
The descriptions of stages 13–15 have been described as insufficiently precise.
[ Ross et al., 2014]
See also
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
References
Literature
*
*
*
*
*
* Biggs, J.B. & Collis, K. (1982). ''Evaluating the quality of learning: The SOLO taxonomy (structure of the observed learning outcome)''. New York: Academic Press.
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
* Fischer, K.W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. ''Psychological Review'', 87(6), 477–531.
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
*
External links
Behavioral Development BulletinSociety for Research in Adult Development
{{DEFAULTSORT:Model of hierarchical complexity
Cognition
Management cybernetics
Complex systems theory
Developmental stage theories
Psychophysics