HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
and
Chiapas Chiapas, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Chiapas, is one of the states that make up the Political divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. It comprises Municipalities of Chiapas, 124 municipalities and its capital and large ...
. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the
United States The United States of America (USA), also known as the United States (U.S.) or America, is a country primarily located in North America. It is a federal republic of 50 U.S. state, states and a federal capital district, Washington, D.C. The 48 ...
and
Mexico Mexico, officially the United Mexican States, is a country in North America. It is the northernmost country in Latin America, and borders the United States to the north, and Guatemala and Belize to the southeast; while having maritime boundar ...
, with notable populations living in
Oakland, California Oakland is a city in the East Bay region of the San Francisco Bay Area in the U.S. state of California. It is the county seat and most populous city in Alameda County, California, Alameda County, with a population of 440,646 in 2020. A major We ...
and
Washington, D.C. Washington, D.C., formally the District of Columbia and commonly known as Washington or D.C., is the capital city and federal district of the United States. The city is on the Potomac River, across from Virginia, and shares land borders with ...
The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's ''A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language'' (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.


Classification

Mam is closely related to the Tektitek language, and the two languages together form the Mamean sub-branch of the Mayan language family. Along with the Ixilan languages, Awakatek and Ixil, these make up the Greater Mamean sub-branch, one of the two branches of the Eastern Mayan languages (the other being the Greater Quichean sub-branch, which consists of 10 Mayan languages, including Kʼicheʼ).


Dialects

Because contact between members of different Mam communities is somewhat limited, the language varies considerably even from village to village. Nevertheless, mutual intelligibility, though difficult, is possible through practice. Mam varieties within Mexico and Guatemala are divided into five dialect groups: *Northern Mam in
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
, Mexico and southern Huehuetenango Department, Guatemala. Northern Mam is the least conservative group according to
Terrence Kaufman Terrence Kaufman (1937 – March 3, 2022) was an American linguist specializing in documentation of unwritten languages, lexicography, Mesoamerican historical linguistics and language contact phenomena. He was an emeritus professor of linguistic ...
. *Southern Mam in southern
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
, Mexico and Quetzaltenango Department, San Marcos Department, and Retalhuleu Department, Guatemala. *Central Mam in
Chiapas Chiapas, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Chiapas, is one of the states that make up the Political divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. It comprises Municipalities of Chiapas, 124 municipalities and its capital and large ...
, Mexico and San Marcos Department, Guatemala. *Western Mam in eastern
Chiapas Chiapas, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Chiapas, is one of the states that make up the Political divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. It comprises Municipalities of Chiapas, 124 municipalities and its capital and large ...
, Mexico and northwestern San Marcos Department, Guatemala. The Tektitek language may be mutually intelligible with Western Mam dialects. *Soconusco Mam in the
Soconusco Soconusco is a region in the southwest corner of the state of Chiapas in southeastern Mexico along its border with Guatemala. It is a narrow strip of land wedged between the Sierra Madre de Chiapas mountains and the Pacific Ocean. It is the sout ...
region,
Chiapas Chiapas, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Chiapas, is one of the states that make up the Political divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. It comprises Municipalities of Chiapas, 124 municipalities and its capital and large ...
,
Mexico Mexico, officially the United Mexican States, is a country in North America. It is the northernmost country in Latin America, and borders the United States to the north, and Guatemala and Belize to the southeast; while having maritime boundar ...
In addition to these, the dialects of Chiapas, Mexico are characterized by significant grammatical as well as lexical differences from the Guatemalan varieties.


Distribution

Mam is spoken in 64 communities in four Guatemalan departments and numerous communities in
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
and Chiapas, Mexico. Neighboring languages include Jakaltek and Qʼanjobʼal to the north, Tektitek and Qato'k to the west, and Ixil, Awakatek, Sipacapense, and Kʼicheʼ to the east. Quetzaltenango Department * San Miguel Sigüilá * Concepción Chiquirichapa * Génova * El Palmar * San Juan Ostuncalco * Cajolá * San Martín Sacatepéquez * Colomba * Flores Costa Cuca * Huitán * Palestina de Los Altos * Cabricán Huehuetenango Department * San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán * Cuilco * Tectitán * San Pedro Necta * San Sebastián Huehuetenango * Malacatancito *
Todos Santos Cuchumatán Todos Santos Cuchumatán () is a Municipalities of Guatemala, municipality in the Guatemalan Departments of Guatemala, department of Huehuetenango Department, Huehuetenango. It is situated in the Sierra de los Cuchumatanes at an elevation of 2,500 ...
* San Rafael Petzal * Colotenango * Santa Bárbara * San Juan Atitán * Aguacatán * San Gaspar Ixchil * La Libertad * La Democracia * Huehuetenango * Chiantla * Santiago Chimaltenango * San Juan Ixcoy San Marcos Department * San Antonio Sacatepéquez * San Lorenzo * Tejutla * San Rafael Pie de La Cuesta * San Pedro Sacatepéquez *
La Reforma In History of Mexico, the history of Mexico, (from Spanish language, Spanish: "The Reform"), or reform laws, refers to a pivotal set of laws, including a Federal Constitution of the United Mexican States of 1857, new constitution, that were ...
* El Quetzal * Sibinal * San José Ojetenam * Pajapita * San Cristobal Cucho * Nuevo Progreso * San Marcos *
Concepción Tutuapa Concepción Tutuapa () is a town and municipality in the San Marcos department of Guatemala. It had a population of 49,363 according to the census of 2002 and of the 2018 census there is a total population of 68,148. History Spanish colony ...
* San Pablo * Ixchiguan * San Miguel Ixtahuacán * Tacaná * Tajumulco * Catarina * Esquipulas Palo Gordo * Malacatán * Río Blanco * Comitancillo Retalhuleu Department * El Xaw * Santa Ines * Sibaná * San Miguelito * Nueva Cajolá * San Roque
Chiapas Chiapas, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Chiapas, is one of the states that make up the Political divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. It comprises Municipalities of Chiapas, 124 municipalities and its capital and large ...
* Motozintla * Tapachula * Acaxman * Bejucal de Ocampo * Frontera Comalapa * Mazapa de Madero * Escuintla * Chicomuselo * Siltepec * La Grandeza * Unión Juárez * Bella Vista * El Porvenir * Cacahoatán * Tuzantán * Tuxtla Chico * Huixtla * Huehuetán * Amatenango de la Frontera * Tuxchamén
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
* Champotón *
Campeche Campeche, officially the Free and Sovereign State of Campeche, is one of the 31 states which, with Mexico City, make up the Administrative divisions of Mexico, 32 federal entities of Mexico. Located in southeast Mexico, it is bordered by the sta ...
* Santo Domingo Kesté * Quetzal-Edzná * Mayatecúm * Gumarcaaj


Phonology


Stress

Mam has weight sensitive stress assignment. Primary stress falls on the long vowel in a word if there is one, e.g. 'work'. Words without a long vowel assign primary stress to the vowel preceding the last glottal stop, e.g. 'dipper'. Words without a long vowel or a glottal stop assign stress to the vowel preceding the last consonant in the root, e.g. 'raccoon'. Stress is not assigned to suffixes or enclitics that do not have long vowels or a glottal stop.


Vowels

Mam has 10
vowel A vowel is a speech sound pronounced without any stricture in the vocal tract, forming the nucleus of a syllable. Vowels are one of the two principal classes of speech sounds, the other being the consonant. Vowels vary in quality, in loudness a ...
s, 5 short and 5 long: *A mid-central vowel is an allophone of a short unstressed vowel that can occur in the syllable following a stressed long vowel.


Consonants

Mam has 27
consonant In articulatory phonetics, a consonant is a speech sound that is articulated with complete or partial closure of the vocal tract, except for the h sound, which is pronounced without any stricture in the vocal tract. Examples are and pronou ...
s, including the
glottal stop The glottal stop or glottal plosive is a type of consonantal sound used in many Speech communication, spoken languages, produced by obstructing airflow in the vocal tract or, more precisely, the glottis. The symbol in the International Phonetic ...
: * Stop sounds /p, t, t͡s, t͡ʃ, t͡ʂ, k, q/ are released with aspiration ʰin word-final position. * Todos Santos Mam has an extended amount of affricate consonants being apical palato-alveolar .


Syllable structure

Most roots take the morphological shape CVC. The only possible root final consonant cluster is -''nC''. Syllables can have up to four consonants in a cluster in any position. Most consonant clusters are the result of vowel dropping and morpheme addition.


Morphology

Mam has two sets of agreement markers, known to Mayanists as Set A and Set B markers, which can appear on both nouns and verbs. Mam uses Set A ( ergative) markers on nouns to mark possessor agreement and on verbs to cross-reference the transitive subject. Mam uses Set B (
absolutive In grammar, the absolutive case (abbreviated ) is the case of nouns in ergative–absolutive languages that would generally be the subjects of intransitive verbs or the objects of transitive verbs in the translational equivalents of nominative� ...
) markers on transitive verbs to cross-reference the object and on intransitive verbs to cross-reference the subject. Below is a table of Set A (ergative) and Set B (absolutive) prefixes from England. Phonologically conditioned allomorphs are as follows. *n- ~ w- **n- /__C **w- /__V *Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k- **k- /potential **tzʼ- /__V initial root, non-potential **tz- /__''uul'' 'arrive here', ''iky 'pass by', non-potential **Ø- /__C, non-potential *-a ~ -ya **-ya /V__ ; In the first person in post-vowel environments, -''ya'' varies freely with -''kyʼa'' and -''y. **-a /C__ Some paradigmatic examples from England (1983) are given below. Note that "Ø-" designates a null prefix. Additionally, ''ma'' is an aspectual word meaning 'recent past'.


The Mam verb complex

Verbs in Mam can include inflection for person, aspect and mode, as well as auxiliaries in the form of directionals. The verb complex has distinct forms for transitive and intransitive verb stems depending in part on whether the complex cross-references one or two arguments. The lexical status of the verb complex is ambiguous. The inflections with vowels are phonologically independent (indicated by spaces). Transitive verb complexes with directionals have a dependent suffix. Two of England's examples of intransitive and transitive verb complexes are shown below. Intransitive verb complex with directional Transitive verb complex with directional Mam extends the Set A (ergative) person markers in the context of focused adverbials and certain subordinate clauses. In these contexts, the Set A markers cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs and both the subject and object of transitive verbs. The following examples show the extended ergative marker /t-/ in bold. Intransitive verb complex with extended ergative marking Transitive verb complex with extended ergative marking
REC:recent past POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic REL:relational noun PAT:patient


Verb morphemes

Transitive verbal affixes *-bʼaa 'transitivizer' (vowel length can also be used) *-laa 'applicative' *-wa 'applicative' *-bʼV 'causative' *-chV 'causative' (variants: -chaa, -chii, -chuu) *-kʼuu 'causative' *-lV 'causative' *-mV 'causative' *-nV 'causative' *-pV 'causative' *-qʼV 'causative' *-saa 'causative' *-tzii 'causative' *-tzʼV 'causative' *-txʼii 'causative' *-wV 'causative' *-najee' 'repetitive' *-'kJ 'processive' *-'tz 'processive imperative' Intransitive verbal affixes *-n 'antipassive' *-Vn 'affect' *-ax 'versive' *-ee' 'versive' *-eet 'passive' *-j 'passive' *-njtz 'passive' *-bʼaj 'processive passive' *-bʼa 'intransitivizer' *-ch 'intransitivizer' *-chaj 'intransitivizer' *-paj 'intransitivizer' *-t 'intransitivizer' *-tzʼaj 'intransitivizer' *-tzʼaq 'intransitivizer' *-' ... -al 'specific termination' Other verbal affixes *-l 'infinitive' Aspects Mam verbs have 6 aspects that are prefixed to the verb root. *ma 'recent past' *o 'past' *ok 'potential' (not obligatory) *n- 'progressive' *x- 'recent past dependent' (used in subordinate clauses) *Ø- 'past dependent' (used in subordinate clauses) Modes *Potential transitive: -a' *Potential intransitive: -l *Imperative: -m (-n before directionals) Directionals Directionals are auxiliary elements in verb phrases. They are derived from intransitive verbs. *xi 'away from' *tzaj 'toward' *ul 'there to here' *pon 'here to there' *kubʼ 'down' *jaw 'up' *el 'out' *ok 'in' *kyaj 'remaining' *aj 'returning from here' *ikyʼ 'passing' *bʼaj 'complete'


Pronouns

Mam has no independent pronouns. Rather, pronouns in Mam always exist as
bound morpheme In linguistics, a bound morpheme is a morpheme (the elementary unit of morphosyntax) that can appear only as part of a larger expression, while a free morpheme (or unbound morpheme) is one that can stand alone. A bound morpheme is a type of bound f ...
s.


Nouns

The Mam language displays
inalienable possession In linguistics, inalienable possession ( abbreviated ) is a type of possession in which a noun is obligatorily possessed by its possessor. Nouns or nominal affixes in an inalienable possession relationship cannot exist independently or be "al ...
. Certain Mam nouns cannot be possessed, such as ''kya'j'' 'sky' and ''che'w'' 'star'. On the other hand, some Mam nouns are always possessed, such as ''t-lokʼ'' 'its root' and ''t-bʼaqʼ'' 'its seed'. Noun phrase structure can be summarized into the following template. : The plural clitic is ''qa''. Noun affixes *aj- 'agent' *aj- 'native' *-l 'agentive' *-eenj 'patient' *-bʼil 'instrumental' *-bʼeen 'resultant locative' *-bʼan 'reminder' *-al 'abstract noun' *-abʼiil 'abstract noun' *-leen 'abstract noun' *-le'n 'abstract noun' *-an 'ordinal' *-bʼji'bʼil 'nominalizer' *-bʼal 'nominalizer' *-bʼatz 'nominalizer' *-l 'nominalizer' *-tl 'nominalizer' *-tz 'nominalizer' Relational noun affixes *-u'n 'agent, instrument, causative' *-ee 'dative, possessive, patient, benefactive' *-i'j 'patient, thematic' *-uukʼal 'instrument, comitative' *-iibʼaj 'reflexive' Locative affixes *-bʼutxʼ 'at the corner' *-i'jla 'around' *-iibʼaj 'over' *-jaqʼ 'under' *-txa'n 'at the edge of' *-txlaj 'beside' *-tzii' 'at the entrance of' *-uj 'in' *-witz 'on top of' *-wi' 'on, at the tip of' Classifiers *jal 'non-human' *nu'xh 'baby' *xhlaaqʼ 'child' *bʼixh 'person of the same status (intimate)' *qʼa 'young man' *txin 'young woman' *ma 'man' *xu'j 'woman' *swe'j 'old man' *xhyaa' 'old woman' *xnuq 'old man (respectful)' *xuj 'old man (respectful)' Measure words Measure words quantify
mass noun In linguistics, a mass noun, uncountable noun, non-count noun, uncount noun, or just uncountable, is a noun with the syntactic property that any quantity of it is treated as an undifferentiated unit, rather than as something with discrete eleme ...
s. *baas 'glassful' (< Spanish ''vaso'') *ma'l 'shot of liquor' *laq 'plateful' *pixh 'piece' *txut 'drop' *ba'uj 'a lot'


Numerals

San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán Mam numbers are as follows. Numbers above twenty are rarely used in Ixtahuacán and are usually only known by elderly speakers. Although the number system would have originally been
vigesimal A vigesimal ( ) or base-20 (base-score) numeral system is based on 20 (number), twenty (in the same way in which the decimal, decimal numeral system is based on 10 (number), ten). ''wikt:vigesimal#English, Vigesimal'' is derived from the Latin a ...
(i.e., base 20), the present-day number system of Ixtahuacán is now
decimal The decimal numeral system (also called the base-ten positional numeral system and denary or decanary) is the standard system for denoting integer and non-integer numbers. It is the extension to non-integer numbers (''decimal fractions'') of th ...
.


Syntax

Mam has both verbal and non-verbal types of sentences. Verbal sentences have verbal predicates, whereas non-verbal sentences have a stative or a locative/existential predicate. Verbal predicates have an aspect marker, while non-verbal predicates do not have aspect marking. Both verbal and non-verbal predicates occur in sentence-initial position unless a focused or topicalized phrase is present.


Verbal predicates

Verbal predicates are either transitive or intransitive according to the number of arguments cross-referenced in the verb complex. The number of arguments cross-referenced by the verb complex is not consistent with the transitivity of the verb root or the number of participants in an event. England notes examples of transitive verb roots that only appear in their antipassive or passive forms where they only cross-reference a single participant. *Transitive verb root with obligatory antipassive voice *Transitive verb root with obligatory passive voice Another possibility is the use of intransitive motion verbs to express transitive events. *Intransitive motion verbs expressing transitive events The basic word order in verbal sentences with two nominal arguments is VSO. Other word orders are not acceptable. If only one argument appears in a transitive sentence and the argument is compatible with either person marker on the verb, it has a patient interpretation. Mam speakers use a higher proportion of intransitive sentences than speakers of other Mayan languages. England and Martin (2003) found a low frequency of transitive sentences in Mam texts. Pye (2017) found a low use of overt subjects in transitive sentences in adults speaking to children. One adult produced overt subjects in 6% of transitive sentences. The same adult produced overt subjects in 41% of intransitive sentences and produced overt objects in 49% of transitive sentences.


Non-verbal predicates

Mam adds Set B person markers to nouns and adjectives to form non-verbal predicates. The following Set B person markers are used for non-verbal predicates (i.e., nouns, adjectives). Also, in statives, ''aa'' can be omitted when the rest of the stative is a non- enclitic (in other words, a separate, independent word). Paradigmatic examples of non-verbal predicates from England (1983) are given below.
REC:recent past AP:antipassive suffix PAS:passive suffix POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic INTENS:intensive REL:relational noun PAT:patient TV:transitive verb IMP:imperative CL:noun classifier


Child Language

An overview of child language acquisition in Mam can be found in Pye (2017). Child language data for Mam challenge many theories of language acquisition and demonstrate the need for more extensive documentation of native American languages. Children acquiring Mam produce a higher proportion of verbs than children acquiring K’iche’, but a lower proportion of verbs compared to children acquiring Wastek and Chol. They produce a higher proportion of intransitive verbs relative to transitive verbs than children acquiring other Mayan languages (Pye, Pfeiler and Mateo Pedro 2017:22). Their high proportion of relational noun production is tied to their frequent use of intransitive verbs. The following examples illustrate the children's use of intransitive verbs to express events with two participants. Ages are shown as (years;months.days). WEN (2;0.2) used the intransitive verb ("go_down") in reference to an event of picking coffee. She used the relational noun phrase to express the agent in an oblique phrase. CRU (2;5.12) used the intransitive verb ("go_out") in reference to an event of taking out an object. She used the relational noun phrase to express the agent. JOS (2;6.17) used the intransitive verb ("finish") in reference to finishing a drink. He used the possessive prefix on the noun ("drink")to express the agent. The examples overturn the hypothesis that children tie their use of transitive verbs to object manipulation events. *WEN (2;0.2) *CRU (2;5.12) *JOS (2;6.17) Two-year-old Mam children produce the consonants . They produce in place of glottalized stops, in place of , in place of and , in place of and , in place of , and in place of . Mam children begin producing ejective consonants after they are three and a half years old. The early production of /t͡ʃ/ and /l/ in Mam, as well as the late production of /s/, overturns predictions that all children have similar phonologies due to articulatory development. The following examples illustrate WEN’s verb complex production. In (1), WEN produced the vowel /a/ from the verb root ("give"), the imperative suffix -n, and the directional suffix -tz as /xh/. (Many directionals have contracted forms as suffixes.). WEN omitted the person enclitic -a. In (2), WEN produced the progressive prefix n-, the vowel /e/ from the verb root ("go out"), a spurious /n/, and the directional suffix -tz as /ch/. The intransitive verb belongs to the class of motion verbs that take directional suffixes. Intransitive verbs outside of the class of motion verbs do not take directional suffixes except in imperative contexts. The verb contracts with the directional suffix -tz to produce the stem ("go out to") in adult speech. WEN’s omission of the person enclitic and production of a spurious consonant overturn the hypothesis that children produce forms that are frequent in adult speech. *WEN (1;9.2) *WEN (1;8.21) The children’s production of the directional suffixes demonstrates their early recognition of the distinction between intransitive and transitive verbs in Mam. This distinction is a core feature of Mam grammar, and underpins the ergative morphology on the verbs and nouns. The semantic diversity of the verbs and positionals overturns the hypothesis that children use prototypical activity scenes as a basis for constructing grammatical categories. The children’s grammatical acumen is best seen in their use of the ergative and absolutive agreement markers on verbs. The children produced the prevocalic allomorphs of the ergative markers in nearly all of their obligatory contexts. They produced the preconsonantal allomorphs of the ergative markers in 20% of their obligatory contexts. Two-year-old Mam children display a remarkable awareness of the contexts for extending the use of ergative markers to cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs. Outside of these contexts, they consistently produced absolutive person markers on intransitive verbs. Three Mam children produced ergative person markers on intransitive verbs in half of the obligatory contexts for extended ergativity. The children’s awareness of the contexts for extended ergative use is all the more remarkable because the contexts are tied to clauses in dependent contexts in which aspect is not overtly marked. The following example shows JOS’s use of extended ergative marking (in bold) on the intransitive verb ("go_in") in a purpose clause headed by the adverb ("so that"). The children’s production of ergative markers on intransitive verbs in dependent contexts overturns the theory that children link ergative markers to the subjects of transitive verbs in all contexts. *JOS (2;6.14) Mam two-year-olds produce sentences with a predicate-initial word order. The children, like adults, rarely produce the subject argument in transitive sentences. The Mam children show an ergative pattern of argument production that similar to the adult pattern.
REC:recent past AP:antipassive suffix PAS:passive suffix POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic INTENS:intensive REL:relational noun PAT:patient TV:transitive verb IMP:imperative CL:noun classifier


References


Notes


Bibliography

* * * * * * * *


Further reading

* * *


External links


A simple Mam – Spanish dictionary
* Robert Sitler's Mam – English dictionar
(DOC)(PDF)



Books from Cholsamaj


at the Archive of the Indigenous Languages of Latin America, containing recordings and transcriptions of narratives and conversations in Mam. {{DEFAULTSORT:Mam Language Agglutinative languages Mayan languages Mam Maya Mesoamerican languages Indigenous languages of Central America Languages of Guatemala Huehuetenango Department Quetzaltenango Department San Marcos Department Verb–subject–object languages Languages of Mexico Indigenous languages of Mexico