HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

English nouns form the largest
category Category, plural categories, may refer to: Philosophy and general uses * Categorization, categories in cognitive science, information science and generally *Category of being * ''Categories'' (Aristotle) *Category (Kant) *Categories (Peirce) * ...
of words in
English English usually refers to: * English language * English people English may also refer to: Peoples, culture, and language * ''English'', an adjective for something of, from, or related to England ** English national ide ...
, both in terms of the number of different words and in terms of how often they are used in typical texts. The three main categories of English nouns are common nouns,
proper nouns A proper noun is a noun that identifies a single entity and is used to refer to that entity (''Africa'', ''Jupiter'', ''Sarah (given name), Sarah'', ''Microsoft)'' as distinguished from a common noun, which is a noun that refers to a Class (philo ...
, and
pronouns In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun (list of glossing abbreviations, abbreviated ) is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase. Pronouns have traditionally been regarded as one of the part of speech, parts o ...
. A defining feature of English nouns is their ability to inflect for
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The original examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual numbers c ...
, as through the plural –''s''
morpheme A morpheme is the smallest meaningful Constituent (linguistics), constituent of a linguistic expression. The field of linguistics, linguistic study dedicated to morphemes is called morphology (linguistics), morphology. In English, morphemes are ...
. English nouns primarily function as the heads of noun
phrases In syntax and grammar, a phrase is a group of words or singular word acting as a grammatical unit. For instance, the English expression "the very happy squirrel" is a noun phrase which contains the adjective phrase "very happy". Phrases can consi ...
, which prototypically function at the
clause In language, a clause is a constituent that comprises a semantic predicand (expressed or not) and a semantic predicate. A typical clause consists of a subject and a syntactic predicate, the latter typically a verb phrase composed of a verb with ...
level as subjects,
objects Object may refer to: General meanings * Object (philosophy), a thing, being, or concept ** Object (abstract), an object which does not exist at any particular time or place ** Physical object, an identifiable collection of matter * Goal, an ...
, and predicative complements. These phrases are the only English phrases whose structure includes determinatives and predeterminatives, which add abstract specifying meaning such as
definiteness In linguistics, definiteness is a semantic feature of noun phrases, distinguishing between referents or senses that are identifiable in a given context (definite noun phrases) and those which are not (indefinite noun phrases). The prototypical d ...
and
proximity Proximity may refer to: * Distance, a numerical description of how far apart objects are * Proxemics, the study of human spatial requirements and the effects of population density * Proximity (2000 film), ''Proximity'' (2000 film), an action/thril ...
. Like nouns in general, English nouns typically
denote In linguistics and philosophy, the denotation of an expression is its literal meaning. For instance, the English word "warm" denotes the property of being warm. Denotation is contrasted with other aspects of meaning including connotation. For insta ...
physical objects, but they also denote actions (e.g., ''get up and have a stretch''), characteristics (e.g., ''this red is lovely''), relations in space (e.g., ''closeness''), and just about anything at all. Taken all together, these features separate English nouns from the language's other lexical categories, such as
adjectives In linguistics, an adjective (abbreviated ) is a word that generally modifies a noun or noun phrase or describes its referent. Its semantic role is to change information given by the noun. Traditionally, adjectives were considered one of the mai ...
and
verbs A verb () is a word (part of speech) that in syntax generally conveys an action (''bring'', ''read'', ''walk'', ''run'', ''learn''), an occurrence (''happen'', ''become''), or a state of being (''be'', ''exist'', ''stand''). In the usual descrip ...
. For the purposes of this article, English nouns include
English pronouns The English pronouns form a relatively small category of words in Modern English whose primary semantic function is that of a pro-form for a noun phrase. Traditional grammars consider them to be a distinct part of speech, while most modern gra ...
, but not
English determiners English determiners (also known as determinatives) are words – such as ''the'', ''a'', ''each'', ''some'', ''which'', ''this'', and ''six'' – that are most commonly used with nouns to specify their referents. The determiners form a closed ...
.


Subtypes

English nouns are classified into three major subtypes as common nouns, proper nouns, and pronouns, each with its own typical syntactic behaviour.Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''A Student's Introduction to English Grammar''. Cambridge UP, 2005. p. 84.


Proper nouns

Proper nouns are a class of words such as ''December'', ''Canada'', ''Leah'', and ''Johnson'' that occur within noun phrases (NPs) that are
proper names A proper noun is a noun that identifies a single entity and is used to refer to that entity (''Africa'', ''Jupiter'', '' Sarah'', ''Microsoft)'' as distinguished from a common noun, which is a noun that refers to a class of entities (''continent, ...
, though not all proper names contain proper nouns (e.g., ''General Electric'' is a proper name with no proper noun). The central cases of proper names, according to ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'', "are expressions which have been conventionally adopted as the name of a particular entity." A prominent category of proper names are the ones assigned to particular people or animals (''Elizabeth'', ''Fido''). Others include particular places (''New Zealand'', ''the United States of America'') and institutions (''Cambridge University'', ''the United States Senate''). While proper names may be realized by multi-word constituents, a proper noun is word-level unit in English. Thus, ''Zealand'', for example, is a proper noun, but ''New Zealand'', though a proper name, is not a proper noun. Unlike some common nouns, proper nouns do not typically show number contrast in English. Most proper nouns in English are singular and lack a plural form, though some may instead be plural and lack a singular form. For example, we typically expect ''Michigan'' but not ''*Michigans'' and ''the Philippines'' but not ''*Philippine''. Proper nouns also differ from common nouns in that they typically lack either a determinative or determinative contrast. For instance, we typically expect ''Michigan'' but not ''*a Michigan'', and though ''the Bahamas'' includes the determinative ''the'', the determinative cannot normally be varied (compare ''*a Bahama'' and ''*some Bahamas''). Finally, proper nouns differ from common nouns in that they typically cannot be modified by restrictive modifiers.Quirk, Randolph, et al. ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language''. Longman, 1985. pp. 288–290. In English, the features that distinguish proper nouns from common nouns do not necessarily apply in the rare situations in which proper nouns lack unique denotation. For example, ''London'' typically refers to a unique place, but someone trying to disambiguate between two places named London might pluralize it (''Which of the Londons are you referring to?''), add a determinative (''Do you mean the'' ''London in Ontario?''), or add a restrictive modifier (''Do you mean the London in Ontario?'').


Pronouns

English pronouns are a closed category of words that have a variety of features distinguishing them from common and proper nouns. Unlike common nouns, pronouns are mostly
deictic In linguistics, deixis (, ) is the use of general words and phrases to refer to a specific time, place, or person in context, e.g., the words ''tomorrow'', ''there'', and ''they''. Words are deictic if their semantic meaning is fixed but their de ...
and anaphoric
pro-forms In linguistics, a pro-form is a type of function word or expression that stands in for (expresses the same content as) another word, phrase, clause or sentence where the meaning is recoverable from the context. They are used either to avoid rep ...
. In the clause ''I like you'', for instance, ''I'' and ''you'' are deictic in that their meanings can only be understood in relation to the context of the utterance. In the clause ''Tell Anne I want to talk to her'', on the other hand, ''her'' is anaphoric in that the pronoun derives its meaning from its antecedent (''Anne'', in this case). Also unlike common nouns, English pronouns show distinctions in case (e.g., ''I'', ''me'', ''mine''), person (e.g., ''I'', ''you'') and gender (e.g., ''he'', ''she''). Though both common nouns and pronouns show number distinction in English, they do so differently: common nouns tend to take an inflectional ending (–''s'') to mark plurals, but pronouns typically do not. (The pronoun ''one'' is an exception, as in ''I like those ones''.) English pronouns are also more limited than common nouns in their ability to take dependents. For instance, while common nouns can often be preceded by a determinative (e.g., ''the car''), pronouns cannot. In English conversation, pronouns are roughly as frequent as other nouns. In fiction, pronouns are about one third of all nouns, and in news and academic English, pronouns are a small minority of nouns (<10%).


Common nouns

Common nouns are defined as those that are neither proper nouns nor pronouns. They are the most numerous and the most frequently used in English. Common nouns can be further divided into count and non-count nouns. A count noun can take a number as its determiner (e.g., ''-20 degrees'', ''zero calories'', ''one cat'', ''two bananas'', ''276 dollars''). These nouns tend to designate individually identifiable entities, whereas a non-count noun designates a continuum or an undifferentiated mass (''air'', ''cheese'', ''lots of gravel'' ''some water'', ''enough heat''). The count and non-count distinction also affects what other determiners can occur with the nouns: singular count nouns can occur with ''a'' but not ''some'' (e.g., ''a chair'' but not usually ''*some chair'') while non-count nouns can occur with ''some'' but not ''a'' (e.g., ''some furniture'' but not ''*a furniture'').Quirk, Randolph, et al. ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language''. Longman, 1985. p. 246. Many common nouns have both count and non-count senses. For example, ''beer'' has a non-count sense in ''she was drinking beer'' but a count sense in ''she drank another beer''.


Morphology


Inflectional

A defining property of English nouns is their ability to inflect for number (i.e., singular or plural).Aarts, Bas. ''Oxford Modern English Grammar''. Oxford UP, 2011. pp. 42–44. In addition to number, English pronouns can inflect for case, a feature shared by some NPs (see discussion of case below) but not common nouns themselves.


Common nouns

Common nouns in English have little inflectional morphology, inflecting only for number. In modern English writing, the plural is usually formed with the –''s'' morpheme, which can be realized phonetically as /s/, /z/, or /əz/. For example, the singular nouns ''cat'', ''dog'', and ''bush'' are pluralized as ''cats'' (''s'' = /s/), ''dogs'' (''s'' = /z/'')'', and ''bushes'' (''es'' = /əz/), respectively. Irregularly, English nouns are marked as plural in other ways, often inheriting the plural morphology of older forms of English or the languages that they are borrowed from. Plural forms from Old English resulted from
vowel mutation In linguistics, umlaut (from German "sound alternation") is a sound change in which a vowel is pronounced more like a following vowel or semivowel. The term ''umlaut'' was originally coined in connection with the study of Germanic languages, as ...
(e.g., ''foot/feet''), adding –''en'' (e.g., ''ox/oxen''), or making no change at all (e.g., ''this sheep/those sheep''). English has also borrowed the plural forms of loanwords from various languages, such as Latin (e.g., ''stimulus/stimuli'') and Greek (e.g., ''criterion/criteria''). Some varieties of English use different methods of marking the plural, many of which fall into one of three patterns. First, the plural morpheme may be absent when another word already indicates that the noun is plural. In the clause ''two girl just left'', for instance, speakers of some varieties would not use the plural morpheme on the noun ''girl'' because the determiner ''two'' already marks the noun phrase as plural. ''Dem'', which is derived from ''them'', is often used without the plural morpheme, as in ''dem book'' (rather than ''dem books''). This method of plural marking occurs in
Gullah The Gullah () are an African Americans, African American ethnic group who predominantly live in the South Carolina Lowcountry, Lowcountry region of the U.S. states of Georgia, Florida, South Carolina, and North Carolina, within the coastal plain ...
and
Caribbean English Caribbean English (CE, CarE) is a set of dialects of the English language which are spoken in the Caribbean and Liberia, most countries on the Caribbean coast of Central America, and Guyana and Suriname on the coast of South America. Caribbe ...
among other varieties. Second, the plural morpheme may be absent specifically in noun phrases denoting weights and measures but not in other situations. Thus, some varieties may produce noun phrases like ''ten mile'' (rather than ''ten miles'') while still using the plural morpheme in other contexts (e.g., ''two girls''). This method of plural marking for weights and measures occurs in certain rural varieties of Southern U.S. English. Third, irregular plural nouns may be regularized and use the –''s'' morpheme. This may happen when the plural is not otherwise marked (e.g., ''sheeps'' for ''sheep''), when the plural is typically marked with a morpheme other than –''s'' (e.g., ''oxes'' for ''oxen''), or when the plural is typically formed through vowel mutation (e.g., ''foots'' for ''feet''). In the case of plurals marked by vowel mutation, some varieties may double mark the plural (e.g., ''feets''). Regularization of plural marking occurs in several Englishes, including
African-American English African-American English (or AAE; also known as Black American English, or Black English in American linguistics) is the set of English sociolects spoken by most Black people in the United States and many in Canada; most commonly, it refers t ...
.
Traditional grammar Traditional grammar (also known as classical grammar) is a framework for the description of the structure of a language. The roots of traditional grammar are in the work of classical Greek and Latin philologists. The formal study of grammar based ...
s suggest that English nouns can also take genitive case endings, as in the –'''s'' in ''the cat's paws''. Grammars informed by modern linguistics, however, analyze this ending as applying to entire noun phrases rather than the nouns themselves. In the phrase ''the cat with brown fur's paws'', for example, the possessor is realized by the entire noun phrase ''the cat with brown fur'', not just the noun ''fur''. This analysis can be illustrated in bracketed notation: * sub>NP_[NP_the_cats_paws.html" ;"title="sub>NP_the_cat.html" ;"title="sub>NP [NP the cat">sub>NP [NP the cats paws">sub>NP_the_cat.html" ;"title="sub>NP [NP the cat">sub>NP [NP the cats paws* [NP [NP the cat with brown fur]'s paws]


Pronouns

Those types that are indisputably pronouns are the personal pronouns, relative pronouns, interrogative pronouns, and reciprocal pronouns. The following table presents the Modern Standard English pronouns (for pronouns in other dialects, see the main article on English pronouns). Nominative case is usually used for subjects (e.g., ''I went'') and accusative for objects (''e.g., Help me''). Reflexives are typically objects when the subject and object are the same person or people. Genitives are used for possession, belonging, sources, ancestry, etc. The independent genitive typically forms a noun phrase all on its own (e.g., ''Mine works''), while the dependent genitive usually occurs together with a head noun on which it depends (e.g., 'My copy''''works''.). Interrogative only (e.g., ''Whose is this?''). Relative ''whose'' is not possible (e.g.,''*This is Kim's, whose we forgot'').


Derivational (for common nouns)

The most common noun-forming suffixes in English are -''tion'', ''
-ism ''-ism'' is a suffix in many English words, originally derived from the Ancient Greek suffix ('), and reaching English through the Latin , and the French . It means "taking side with" or "imitation of", and is often used to describe philosoph ...
'', -''ity'', and '' -ness''.Douglas, Biber ''Longman Student Grammar of Spoken and Written English''. Longman, 2011. pp. 320–322. For example, the verb ''activate'' + ''-tion'' becomes the noun ''activation''. English nouns can also be formed by conversion (no change, e.g., ''run''
erb Erb, ERB or ErB may refer to: People * Edgar Rice Burroughs (1875–1950), American author * Erb of Gwent (–), king of Gwent and Glywysing Surname * Abraham Erb (1772–1830), American-born Canadian settler * Albrecht Erb (1628–1714), Austr ...
→ ''run''
oun Oun or OUN may refer to People * Ahmed Oun (born '1946), Libyan major general * Ek Yi Oun (1910–2013), Cambodian politician * Kham-Oun I (1885–1915), Lao queen consort * Õun, an Estonian surname; notable people with this surname * Oun Kham (18 ...
and compounding (putting two bases together, e.g., ''grand'' + ''mother'' → ''grandmother''). There are also many prefixes that can be attached to English nouns to change their meaning. A small list of examples include ''anti-'', ''bi-'', ''dis-'', ''hyper-'', ''mega-'', ''non-'', & ''re-'' (e.g., ''re-'' + vision → ''revision'').


Semantics of nouns and noun phrases

English noun phrases typically inherit the denotation of the head noun. On top of this, they may have many other semantic characteristics including definiteness,
reference Reference is a relationship between objects in which one object designates, or acts as a means by which to connect to or link to, another object. The first object in this relation is said to ''refer to'' the second object. It is called a ''name'' ...
, specificity,
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The original examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual numbers c ...
, quantification,
gender Gender is the range of characteristics pertaining to femininity and masculinity and differentiating between them. Depending on the context, this may include sex-based social structures (i.e. gender roles) and gender identity. Most cultures u ...
, and
person A person ( : people) is a being that has certain capacities or attributes such as reason, morality, consciousness or self-consciousness, and being a part of a culturally established form of social relations such as kinship, ownership of property, ...
.


Denotation and reference

English nouns prototypically
denote In linguistics and philosophy, the denotation of an expression is its literal meaning. For instance, the English word "warm" denotes the property of being warm. Denotation is contrasted with other aspects of meaning including connotation. For insta ...
entities. The denotation of an expression is its literal meaning, such as those meanings listed within monolingual dictionaries.Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language''. Cambridge UP, 2002. pp. 399–400. For example, one of the things that ''apple'' denotes is "a common, round fruit produced by the tree ''Malus domestica'', cultivated in temperate climates." English noun phrases can also refer to entities. A noun phrase is referential if it is used to pick out an entity that is distinguished by properties other those inherent in the meaning of the noun phrase itself. For instance, the noun phrase ''his dog'' in ''Sam found his dog'' picks out a particular entity (a dog) that is distinguishable by properties not expressed in the meaning of ''dog'' (such as breed, color, and the like). Not all noun phrases refer. In fact, some kinds of noun phrases are inherently non-referential. These include negative, interrogative, and bare role noun phrases as well as noun phrases with ''either'' or ''each'' functioning as a determinative. The underlined NPs in the following examples do not refer: # Negative: ''Nobody came''. # Interrogative: ''Who likes ice cream?'' # Bare role: ''She was elected president''. # ''Either'' as determinative: ''Either team might win the game.'' # ''Each'' as determinative: ''She interviewed each child in turn''. #
Dummy pronoun A dummy pronoun is a deictic pronoun that fulfills a syntactical requirement without providing a contextually explicit meaning of its referent. As such, it is an example of exophora. Dummy pronouns are used in many Germanic languages, includin ...
: ''It's raining.'' # Existential ''there'': ''There's a problem.''


Countability and number

Common nouns may be divided into count nouns and non-count nouns. English nouns typically have both count and non-count senses, though for a given noun one sense typically dominates. For example, ''apple'' is usually countable (''two apples''), but it also has a non-count sense (e.g., ''this pie is full of apple''). When discussing different types of something, a count form is available for almost any noun (e.g., ''This shop carries many cheeses''. = "many types of cheese").Gillon, Brendan S. “The Lexical Semantics of English Count and Mass Nouns.” ''Breadth and Depth of Semantic Lexicons'', edited by Evelyne Viegas, Springer, 1999, pp. 19–37, doi:10.1007/978-94-017-0952-1_2. Non-count nouns denote things that, when put together, remain the same thing. For example, if I have luggage and you give me more luggage, I still just have luggage. Count nouns fail this test: if you have an apple, and I give you more apple or more apples, you no longer just have an apple. Modern English marks a division between singular and plural
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The original examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual numbers c ...
. (Old English pronouns also marked the
dual Dual or Duals may refer to: Paired/two things * Dual (mathematics), a notion of paired concepts that mirror one another ** Dual (category theory), a formalization of mathematical duality *** see more cases in :Duality theories * Dual (grammatical ...
number.) Singular number restricts the denotation of the noun to the set of singularities. Plural number is often said to mean more than one, but, in fact, it restricts the denotation of the noun to the set of non-singularities. That is, in English, plural nouns are appropriate for quantities denoted by all the
real numbers In mathematics, a real number is a number that can be used to measure a ''continuous'' one-dimensional quantity such as a distance, duration or temperature. Here, ''continuous'' means that values can have arbitrarily small variations. Every real ...
, including 0 and other quantities smaller than 1, except exactly ±1. The semantic number and grammatical number of a particular NP may not match. For example, with
collective nouns In linguistics, a collective noun is a word referring to a collection of things taken as a whole. Most collective nouns in everyday speech are not specific to one kind of thing. For example, the collective noun "group" can be applied to people (" ...
such as ''committee'', which denote a unit composed of multiple individuals, agreement can either be singular because the noun is morphologically singular (e.g., ''The committee has not yet come to a decision'') or plural because it is semantically plural (e.g.,''The committee have not yet come to a decision'').


Gender and animacy

Modern English has lost the system of
grammatical gender In linguistics, grammatical gender system is a specific form of noun class system, where nouns are assigned with gender categories that are often not related to their real-world qualities. In languages with grammatical gender, most or all nouns ...
that was present in Old English, and while there is some disagreement over what has replaced it, generally speaking English is said to have a system of "natural gender", which applies only to the pronouns. Curzan, Anne. ''Gender Shifts in the History of English''. Cambridge UP, 2003. A natural gender is one "in which there is a clear correlation between masculine and feminine nouns and biological traits in the referent." But whether this accurately characterizes the English gender system is disputed. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' argues that English has a "weakly grammaticalized" gender, which is based only on pronoun agreement. This gender system involves two subsystems: one involving the distinctions between the personal pronouns ''he'', ''she'', and ''it'' and another involving the distinctions between the relative pronouns ''who'' and ''which''. In the personal pronoun subsystem, nouns can be classified according to whether they are compatible with one, two, or three of these three personal pronouns. Single-gender and dual-gender nouns can be subclassified according to which specific pronouns they agree with. This results in seven classes:Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language''. Cambridge UP, 2002. pp. 489–491. * Single-gender masculine nouns (e.g., ''boy'', ''stepson'') * Single-gender feminine nouns (e.g., ''girl'', ''stepdaughter'', ''actress'') * Single-gender neuter nouns (e.g., ''arrival'', ''beer'') * Dual-gender masculine/feminine nouns (e.g., ''actor'', ''doctor'') * Dual-gender masculine/neuter nouns (e.g., ''bull'', ''brother'') * Dual-gender feminine/neuter nouns (e.g., ''cow'', ''sister'', ''ship'') * Triple-gender nouns (e.g., ''baby'', ''dog'') These classes are not equally common. For instance, single-gender neuter nouns account for a large majority of common nouns while dual-gender masculine/neuter nouns account for only male animal species and certain kinship terms that can apply to both humans and animals. In the relative pronoun subsystem, nouns can be classified according to whether they agree with ''who'' or ''which''. Nouns that agree with ''who'' are called personal (or
animate Animation is a method by which still figures are manipulated to appear as moving images. In traditional animation, images are drawn or painted by hand on transparent celluloid sheets to be photographed and exhibited on film. Today, most anima ...
) nouns while nouns that agree with ''which'' are called non-personal (or inanimate) nouns.Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language''. Cambridge UP, 2002. pp. 497–499. Though there is substantial overlap between nonpersonal nouns and neuter nouns and between personal nouns and masculine and feminine nouns, the overlaps are not perfect. For instance, a ship can agree with either ''it'' or ''she'' but can only agree with ''which'' (not ''who''). Similarly, ''which'' can serve as an antecedent to ''he'' or ''she'', as in ''there is a dog which attacked his/her owner''.


The syntax of nouns and noun phrases

Some defining properties of English nouns are that they function as the heads of NPs and that they can be specified by determinatives and modified by pre-head adjective phrases. A defining property of English NPs is that they prototypically function at the clause level as subjects, objects, and predicative complements.Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language''. Cambridge UP, 2002. pp. 326–328.


Functions

English nouns function as the head of a nominal (see §Internal structure below), which in turn mostly functions as the head of an NP. At the clause-level, English NPs typically function as subjects, objects, and predicative complements. The following table shows these typical functions and the other functions NPs can take: Nominals (see §Internal structure, below), also appear as pre-head modifier in a nominal (e.g., ''a two day conference'').


Internal structure

A simple noun phrase like ''some good ideas'' has a head nominal, a phrase that excludes any determinative (here, ''some''), and that nominal, in turn, has a head noun (here ''ideas'') along with any modifiers or complements. Roughly speaking, the nominal includes everything after the determinative (similar to the way a clause has a verb phrase that includes basically everything after the subject). The following
tree In botany, a tree is a perennial plant with an elongated stem, or trunk, usually supporting branches and leaves. In some usages, the definition of a tree may be narrower, including only woody plants with secondary growth, plants that are ...
shows the internal structure of an NP with all the main types of dependents:
modifiers In linguistics, a modifier is an optional element in phrase structure or clause structure which ''modifies'' the meaning of another element in the structure. For instance, the adjective "red" acts as a modifier in the noun phrase "red ball", provi ...
, a
determinative A determinative, also known as a taxogram or semagram, is an ideogram used to mark semantic categories of words in logographic scripts which helps to disambiguate interpretation. They have no direct counterpart in spoken language, though they may ...
, a predeterminative (labeled here as a kind of modifier), and a
complement A complement is something that completes something else. Complement may refer specifically to: The arts * Complement (music), an interval that, when added to another, spans an octave ** Aggregate complementation, the separation of pitch-class ...
. (The triangles are a convention to simplify the representation of the inner structures of phrases when it is less relevant.)


Determinatives

A basic English NP splits into an optional
determinative A determinative, also known as a taxogram or semagram, is an ideogram used to mark semantic categories of words in logographic scripts which helps to disambiguate interpretation. They have no direct counterpart in spoken language, though they may ...
(usually a
determiner phrase In linguistics, a determiner phrase (DP) is a type of phrase headed by a determiner such as ''many''. Controversially, many approaches, take a phrase like ''not very many apples'' to be a DP, headed, in this case, by the determiner ''many''. This is ...
or a genitive NP) and a head nominal (e.g., 'many'' 'good people''. In the diagram above, the determinative is ''the'', and the head nominal is ''preposterous ideas about exercise that Bill has''. The determinative, if present, always precedes the nominal and is licensed by the head noun. That is, it must agree in number and countability (e.g., ''many people'', *''many person'', ''some police'', ''*a police'') with the head noun. Though the determinative function is typically realized by determiner phrases, they may also be realized by other phrases. Noun phrases that realize the determinative function are typically in the genitive case (e.g., ''your interview'') but do not need to be (e.g., ''this size home''). Determiners can also be realized by prepositional phrases, such as ''up to a dozen'' in the noun phrase ''up to a dozen agencies''.


Predeterminatives

Inside the NP, but outside the nominal, there are also predeterminatives, as exemplified by ''all'' in the tree diagram above. In this case, ''all'' has a specifying role rather than a modifying role in the noun phrase, much like the determinative ''the'', but the determinative function has already been filled. To account for noun phrases like these, some grammars (such as ''Oxford Modern English Grammar'' and ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language'') also recognize the function of predeterminative (or predeterminer). Other grammars offer different accounts of these constructions. For example, the ''Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' classifies them as a "predeterminer modifier". Like the determinative function, the predeterminative function is typically realized by determiner phrases. However, they can also be realized by noun phrases (e.g., ''three times the speed'') and adverb phrases (e.g., ''twice the population'').


Modifiers

Inside the nominal, modifiers can be divided into pre-head (before the noun) and post-head (after the noun).
Adjective phrase An adjective phrase (or adjectival phrase) is a phrase whose head is an adjective. Almost any grammar or syntax textbook or dictionary of linguistics terminology defines the adjective phrase in a similar way, e.g. Kesner Bland (1996:499), Crystal (1 ...
s are the prototypical pre-head modifiers of nouns, as exemplified by ''preposterous'' in the tree diagram above. Adjective-like prepositional phrases can also function as pre-head modifiers of nouns. For example, the prepositional phrase ''under threat'' functions as a pre-head modifier in the noun phrase ''the under-threat postal service''. The adjective-like nature of these prepositional phrases is indicated by the tendency for them to be hyphenated in writing and the fact that they can typically be paraphrased with an adjective phrase (compare ''the endangered postal service'').Aarts, Bas. ''Oxford Modern English Grammar''. Oxford UP, 2011. p. 126–129. Similarly, adjective-like clauses can function as pre-head modifiers of nouns. In the noun phrase ''pay-as-you-go SIM card'', for instance, the clause ''pay as you go'' functions as a pre-head modifier. Like the adjective-like prepositions, these clauses tend to be hyphenated in writing. Other pre-head modifiers of nouns include nominals. In the noun phrase ''Nirvana's classic early nineties album'', for example, the nominal ''early nineties'' modifies the noun ''album''. The nominal's status a modifier can be made clearer by paraphrasing the noun phrase that contains it: ''Nirvana's classic album from the early nineties'', in which ''from the early nineties'' is more clearly a modifier. Verb phrases can also function as pre-head modifiers of nouns. For instance, the verb phrase ''regularly dripping'' can function as a pre-head modifier in the noun phrase ''a regularly dripping faucet''. The fact that ''dripping'' can be and is modified by a manner adverb (''regularly'') but cannot be modified by a degree adverb (such as ''very'') indicates that these pre-head modifiers are verb phrases rather than adjective phrases because verbs can typically be modified by manner but not degree adverbs while adjectives can typically be modified by degree but not manner adverbs. Another pre-head modifier of nouns is determiner phrases. For example, the determiner phrase ''two'' in the noun phrase ''these two images'' functions as a pre-head modifier. While determiners that occur before nouns tend to function as determinatives, noun phrases can contain only one determinative, so additional determiner phrases must have some other function. In ''these two images'', the determiner phrase ''these'' fills the determinative function, so the additional determiner phrase ''two'' must instead be analyzed as a pre-head modifier. Some grammars label these determiner phrases ''postdeterminers''. Rarely, an adverb phrase can function as a pre-head modifier of nouns. In the noun phrase ''an almost victory'', for example, the adverb phrase ''almost'' functions as a pre-head modifier.
Relative clause A relative clause is a clause that modifies a noun or noun phraseRodney D. Huddleston, Geoffrey K. Pullum, ''A Student's Introduction to English Grammar'', CUP 2005, p. 183ff. and uses some grammatical device to indicate that one of the arguments ...
s, as exemplified by ''that Bill has'' in the tree diagram above, are common as post-head modifiers. Prepositional phrases are another common variety of post-head modifier. In the noun phrase ''an apple in a tree'', for example, the prepositional phrase ''in a tree'' functions as a post-head modifier. Adjective phrases can also function as post-head modifiers. Some of these adjective phrases are reduced relative clauses, such as ''balloons full of helium'' (compare ''balloons that were full of helium''). Others are post-positive adjective phrases, such as ''the attorney general''. Noun phrases themselves can function as post-head adjuncts in noun phrases. In the noun phrase ''shoes that size'', for instance, the noun phrase ''that size'' functions as a post-head modifier. Certain determiners (namely, ''each'', ''enough'', ''less'', and ''more'') can head determiner phrases that function as post-head modifiers of noun phrases, as in the determiner phrase ''each'' in ''three dollars each''. Rarely, adverb phrases can function as post-head modifiers, such as the adverb phrase ''soon'' in the noun phrase ''some day soon''. External modifiers exist inside the NP but outside the nominal. These modifiers are often adverb phrases, as exemplified by ''even'' in the tree diagram above. External modifiers can also be realized by prepositional phrases (e.g., ''by far the greatest ally'') and noun phrases (e.g., ''every bit a philosopher''). External modifiers can only attach to the beginnings or ends of noun phrases. When positioned at the beginning, they occur before any predeterminative, determinative, or internal modifier. In the noun phrase ''even all their best songs'', for instance, the external modifier (''even'') must occur before the predeterminative (''all''), determinative (''their''), and internal modifier (''best''). Some external modifiers can move freely between the beginning and the end of their noun phrase. For example, ''by far the greatest ally'' can also be written ''the greatest ally by far''.


Complements

A nominal can occasionally include a complement, a dependent licensed by the head noun. Usually, these are prepositional phrases or
subordinate clauses A subordinate clause, dependent clause, subclause, or embedded clause is a clause that is embedded within a complex sentence. For instance, in the English sentence "I know that Bette is a dolphin", the clause "that Bette is a dolphin" occurs as th ...
. The head of such prepositional phrases is typically ''of'', as in ''our review of your application'' or ''your receipt of the envelope''. In some of these cases, the complement and noun can be compared to a verb and direct object pair (''we reviewed your application''; ''you received the envelope''). In other cases, the head is not ''of'', as exemplified by ''about exercise'' in the tree diagram above. Clauses that function as complements in noun phrases can be either finite (''a realization that it is important'') or non-finite (''a requirement for them to do it''). As with prepositional phrase complements of nouns, certain clause complements of nouns can be compared to verb and complement pairs (''they realized that it is important''; ''somebody required them to do it''). Nouns can also be complemented by noun phrases. Unusually, these noun phrase complements occur before the head noun. For example, the noun phrase ''kinesiology'' functions as a pre-head complement in the larger noun phrase ''a kinesiology student''. The noun phrase's status a complement can be made clearer by paraphrasing the noun phrase that contains it: ''a student of kinesiology'', in which ''of kinesiology'' is more clearly a complement. When there is a complement, usually there's only one, but up to three are possible (e.g., ''a bet for $10 with DJ that it wasn't true''.)


Order of elements in noun phrases

''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' proposes the following rigid order of elements within noun phrases: pre-head external modifiers (peripheral modifiers and predeterminatives), determinatives, pre-head internal modifiers, pre-head complement, head, post-head internal dependents, and post-head external modifiers (emphatic reflexives and focusing modifiers).Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''A Student's Introduction to English Grammar''. Cambridge UP, 2005. pp. 452–455. These elements are present in the example below: These ordering constraints are called rigid because violating them results in an ungrammatical noun phrase. For example, ''the very happy linguistics students'' could not become ''the linguistics very happy students''. Other ordering constraints are labile, meaning that they reflect the general order of things but may be violated without producing an ungrammatical phrase. For example, pre-head internal modifiers that indicate age typically occur before those that indicate color (e.g. ''the new blue tie''), but this order can be violated for various reasons without producing an ungrammatical phrase (e.g., ''The blue new tie'' is a possible answer to the question ''Which new tie will you wear?''). Pre-head internal modifiers and post-head internal dependents are subject to labile ordering constraints.


Order of pre-head internal modifiers

Much attention has been given to the order of pre-noun internal modifiers in both academic and popular writings on English grammar. Many proposed orders appeal to semantic categories. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'', for example, proposes the following order for residual pre-head modifiers: evaluative (e.g., ''good'', ''annoying''), general property (e.g., ''big'', ''cruel''), age (e.g., ''new'', ''ancient''), color (e.g., ''black'', ''crimson''), provenance (e.g., ''French'', ''Chinese''), manufacture (e.g., ''cotton'', ''carved''), type (e.g., ''passenger aircraft'', ''men's department'').
Mark Forsyth Mark Forsyth (born 2 April 1977) is a British writer of non-fiction who came to prominence with a series of books concerning the meaning and etymology of English words. He is the author of best-selling books '' The Etymologicon'', '' The Horol ...
suggests that adjectives must occur in the following order: opinion, size, age, shape, color, origin, material, purpose.Liberman, Mark. "Big Bad Modifier Order." ''Language Log'', Institute for Research in Cognitive Science, U of Pennsylvania, 4 Sept. 2016, 8:08 a.m.
languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=27890.
/ref> These orders are similar to the order as analyzed by Charles Darling, which offers this order: observation, size, shape, age, color, origin, material, qualifier. The following table summarizes these orders: These ordering constraints correctly predict noun phrases like ''a beautiful old Italian touring car'', in which ''beautiful'' is evaluative (or opinion/observation), ''old'' an age, ''Italian'' an origin (or provenance), and ''touring'' a type (or purpose/qualifier). However, Mark Liberman notes that these ordering constraints can lead to incorrect predictions: ''ugly'' is an opinion and ''big'' a size, but corpus data shows that ''big ugly'' is far more common than ''ugly big''. Liberman also notes that these orders fail to account for strong preferences within categories. For example, ''long'' and ''tall'' are both sizes, but ''long tall'' is generally preferred to ''tall long''. Stefanie Wulff summarizes and evaluates a variety of other factors that predict the order of pre-head modifiers in English noun phrases. From a phonological perspective, shorter modifiers typically occur before longer ones, other things being equal. For example, ''the long intelligent book'' is generally preferred to ''the intelligent long book''. From a semantic perspective, the more inherent qualities of a thing tend to occur closer to the noun. For instance, ''solid stainless steel'' is generally preferred to ''stainless solid steel'' because the stainlessness of stainless steel is more inherent than the solidness of solid steel. Also from a semantic perspective, modifiers that "are less dependent on comparison are put nearer to the head noun." For example, the redness of a file can be determined without comparing it to another file but the smallness of a file can only be determined by comparison with another file. Thus, ''a small red file'' is generally preferable to ''a red small file''. From a pragmatic perspective, modifiers that "are remembered most easily upon the occurrence of the noun" tend to occur closer to the noun. For instance, ''blonde'' tends to be more closely associated with hair than ''nice'' is, so ''nice blonde hair'' is more likely than ''blonde nice hair''. Also from a pragmatic perspective, more frequently used modifiers tend to occur before less frequently used modifiers. For example, ''big'' is a more frequently used word than ''cold'', so we would expect ''a big cold'' lake rather than ''a cold big lake''. Some grammars have proposed multiple "zones" for pre-head modifiers in English noun phrases. ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language'', for example, proposes four "premodification zones." The pre-central zone is filled by nongradable adjectives, particularly intensifiers such as ''major'' and ''numerous''. The central zone consists of the most prototypical adjectives, that is, adjectives that admit intensifiers and comparison and can also appear in predicate position. Within this central zone, evaluative adjectives typically occur first, and the usual order for the rest is nonderived adjectives, then adjectives derived from verbs, and finally adjectives derived from nouns. The post-central zone includes participles and color terms. The pre-head zone includes adjectives denoting provenance, adjectives with the meaning of "relating to (noun)" (such as ''annual'' and ''political''), and nouns. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' proposes two zones: early pre-head modifiers and residual pre-head modifiers. Early pre-head modifiers include cardinal and ordinal numbers numerals (e.g., ''two'', ''second''), superlative adjectives (e.g., ''largest'', ''youngest''), and primacy adjectives (e.g., ''key'', ''primary''). Residual pre-head modifiers include all other pre-head internal modifiers.


Order of post-head internal dependents

The category "post-head internal dependents" includes post-head modifiers and complements. Though modifiers tend not to occur between complements and their heads, ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' does not characterize this tendency as a rigid ordering constraint because the order is also affected by the weight of the constituent, with lighter dependents typically occurring before heavy dependents. In the noun phrase ''the rumor in the city that Minakshi had decreed that no white woman could live for long within sight of her temple'', for example, the modifier ''in the city'' separates the head ''rumor'' from the complement ''that Minakshi had decreed that no white woman could live for long within sight of her temple'' because the complement is relatively heavy while the modifier is relatively light.


Nouns versus other lexical categories


Adjectives

Nouns and adjectives in English can generally be distinguished by their grammatical features: Prototypical nouns can inflect for number while adjectives cannot. Prototypical adjectives can inflect for degree of
comparison Comparison or comparing is the act of evaluating two or more things by determining the relevant, comparable characteristics of each thing, and then determining which characteristics of each are similar to the other, which are different, and t ...
while nouns cannot. Prototypical nouns head phrases that can function as subject, direct object, and indirect object while prototypical adjectives head phrases that can function as pre-head modifier of nouns and subject-related complement. Prototypical adjectives can be modified by ''very'' while nouns cannot. Nouns can head phrases containing determinatives and predeterminatives while adjectives cannot. The following table summarizes some of these characteristics: In noun phrases such as ''the boy actor'', words like ''boy'' do not fall neatly into the categories noun or adjective. ''Boy'' is more like an adjective than a noun in that it functions as a pre-head modifier of a noun, which is a function prototypically filled by adjective phrases, and in that that it cannot be pluralized in this position (*''the boys actor''). However, ''boy'' is more like a noun than an adjective in that it cannot be modified by ''very'' (*''the very boy actor'') as adjectives typically can be and in that it cannot be separated from the head noun by an adjective (*''the boy talented actor''). Further, ''boy'' is more like a noun in that it cannot occur alone as a subject-related predicative complement (*''the actor is boy'').Aarts, Bas. ''Syntactic Gradience: The Nature of Grammatical Indeterminacy.'' Oxford UP, 2007. pp. 129–136. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' classifies words like ''boy'' as nouns.
John Robert Ross John Robert "Haj" Ross (born May 7, 1938) is an American poet and linguist. He played a part in the development of generative semantics (as opposed to interpretive semantics) along with George Lakoff, James D. McCawley, and Paul Postal. He wa ...
similarly classifies it as an "adjectival noun", a noun with some adjectival properties. Color terms also exhibit features of both nouns and adjectives. In many cases, the category of these terms can be clearly identified. For example, color terms used as subjects (''blue represents hope'') or complements (''my favorite color is blue'') appear to be typical nouns while color terms occurring attributively (''the blue light'') appear to be typical adjectives. Similarly, color terms marked as plural (''the blues in his paintings'') appear to be nouns while those marked as comparative (''bluer'') or superlative (''bluest'') appear to be adjectives. However, James D. McCawley notes a case in which color terms appear to have features of nouns and adjectives at the same time: ''a deep blue necktie''. In this case, the modifier of ''blue'' is an adjective (''deep'') rather than an adverb (''deeply''), which suggests that the color term is a noun. However, its function appears to be the same as the ''blue'' in ''the blue light'', which is an adjective. Bas Aarts notes that this apparent dual categorization can be avoided by treating phrases like ''deep blue'' as adjective-adjective compounds. Phrases like ''the lucky'' in ''the lucky don't need to diet'' also present challenges. Words like ''lucky'' in this case have features typical of a noun; specifically, they appear to head phrases that (1) contain determinatives and (2) have the prototypical functions of noun phrases (such as subject, in this example). However, these words also have features of adjectives. For instance, they can be modified by ''very'' (''the very lucky don't need to diet'') and combine with morphemes that can typically attach only to adjectives, such as ''un-'' (''the unlucky must diet''). Complicating matters further, they can take as pre-head modifiers either adjectives (''the ostentatious rich'') or adverbs (t''he completely innocent''). Aarts argues that phrases like these are best analyzed as noun phrases with an empty element functioning as the head, yielding an analysis like this: sub>NP_the_[AP_completelyAdv_innocentAdjN.html" ;"title="sub>AP completelyAdv innocentAdj">sub>NP the [AP completelyAdv innocentAdjN">sub>AP completelyAdv innocentAdj">sub>NP the [AP completelyAdv innocentAdjN ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' offers a similar analysis, calling words like ''lucky'' and ''innocent'' in these cases "fused modifier-heads". In other words, they treat these words as adjectives that have fused with an unexpressed head.


Verbs

In English, nouns and verbs can typically be distinguished according to their grammatical features: Prototypical nouns can inflect for number while verbs cannot. Verbs take a variety of inflectional endings that nouns cannot, such as the ''-ing'' suffix of the present participle form. Nouns typically take prepositional phrases and clauses as complements while verbs typically take noun phrases and clauses as complements. The typical pre-head modifiers of nouns are adjective phrases, but the typical pre-head modifiers of verbs are adverb phrases. Nouns can head phrases containing determinatives and predeterminatives while verbs cannot.Aarts, Bas. ''Syntactic Gradience: The Nature of Grammatical Indeterminacy.'' Oxford UP, 2007. pp. 143–145. The following table summarizes some of these characteristics: Certain words derived from nouns, specifically those ending in ''-ing'' (such as ''painting''), can share features of both nouns and verbs. ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language'' illustrates the gradience from verbal nouns to verbs in their present participle forms, with the earlier examples behaving more like nouns and the later examples behaving more like verbs:Quirk, Randolph, et al. ''A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language''. Longman, 1985. pp. 1290–1292. # some paintings of Brown’s # Brown’s paintings of his daughters # The painting of Brown is as skillful as that of Gainsborough. # Brown’s deft painting of his daughter is a delight to watch. # Brown’s deftly painting his daughter is a delight to watch. # I dislike Brown’s painting his daughter # I dislike Brown painting his daughter (when she ought to be at school) # I watched Brown painting his daughter. # Brown deftly painting his daughter is a delight to watch. # Painting his daughter, Brown noticed that his hand was shaking. # Brown painting his daughter that day, I decided to go for a walk. # The man painting the girl is Brown. # The silently painting man is Brown. # Brown is painting his daughter. ''Painting(s)'' in [1]– are unambiguously nouns. ''Paintings'' in [1] and [2] feature the plural ''-s'' morpheme associated with nouns and also head phrases containing determinatives (i.e., ''some'' and ''Brown's''), a feature also observed in ''Painting'' in is also modified by an adjective phrase (''deft''), further suggesting that it is a noun. Meanwhile, ''painting'' in 0 4are unambiguously verbs. Of these, all but 3take post-head noun phrase complements, a feature of verbs but not nouns. While the ''painting'' in 3does not take a noun phrase complement, it is modified by an adverb phrase (''silently''), a feature typical of verbs that is also present in and The troublesome cases are the ones represented by the ''painting''s in which demonstrate features of both nouns and verbs. These are often called
gerunds In linguistics, a gerund ( abbreviated ) is any of various nonfinite verb forms in various languages; most often, but not exclusively, one that functions as a noun. In English, it has the properties of both verb and noun, such as being modifiable ...
(though the terminology can vary). The ''painting''s in are noun-like in that they are the heads of phrases functioning as either subject of direct object. The ''painting''s in and are even more noun-like in that they occur with the determinative ''Brown's''. However, the ''painting''s in are also verb-like in that they take a post-head noun phrase complement. The ''painting'' in is even more verb-like in that it is modified by the adverb phrase ''deftly''. Linguists have offered a variety of accounts for English gerunds. For instance, Geoffrey K. Pullum and James P. Blevins both argue that gerunds are noun phrases with verb phrase heads. Other linguists, such as Richard Hudson, argue that gerunds are both verbs and nouns. Yet others, such as Bas Aarts, argue that the fact that gerunds tend to occur in the same places as noun phrases (as subject, direct object, and so on) is not enough to support that they occur within noun phrases and instead treat them as verbs that happen to be in non-canonical positions.


Adverbs

There is typically little confusion between nouns and adverbs in English because there is no overlap in the inflectional morphology that they take (''-s'' for nouns, ''-er'' and ''-est'' for adverbs) and they tend to cooccur with different kinds of words (e.g., nouns can head phrases containing determinatives while adverbs cannot). Further, nouns and adverbs tend to head phrases with different prototypical functions: noun phrases typically function as subjects, direct objects, and indirect objects while adverb phrases typically function as adjuncts. Despite no overlap in the form and distribution of nouns and adverbs, some linguists suggest gradience between a certain class of nouns and adverbs. For example, Barbara M. H. Strang notes that words such as ''yesterday'' and ''today'' have features of both nouns and adverbs. They are noun like in that they can occupy typical noun phrase positions and head possessive noun phrases (e.g., ''yesterday's news''), but unlike prototypical nouns, they cannot be made plural and do not head phrases contain determinatives. Bas Aarts notes that this argument does not actually assert any adverb-like properties but rather just a lack of certain properties of nouns, suggesting that words like ''yesterday'' and ''today'' are nouns, albeit less prototypical than some nouns.


Determiners

There is typically little confusion between nouns and determiners in English, but certain words, namely ''you'' and ''we'', share features of both pronouns and determiners in certain constructions, as in ''we students know the truth''. These words resemble pronouns in that they show case contrast (compare ''us students''), a feature that, in Modern English, is typical of pronouns but not determiners.Aarts, Bas. ''Syntactic Gradience: The Nature of Grammatical Indeterminacy''. Oxford UP, 2007. p. 125. Because they resemble pronouns in this way, Evelyne Delorme and Ray C. Dougherty treat words like ''us'' as pronouns in
apposition Apposition is a grammatical construction in which two elements, normally noun phrases, are placed side by side so one element identifies the other in a different way. The two elements are said to be ''in apposition'', and one of the elements is c ...
with the noun phrases that follow them, which is an analysis that ''
Merriam–Webster's Dictionary of English Usage ''Merriam-Webster's Dictionary of English Usage'' is a usage dictionary published by Merriam-Webster, Inc., of Springfield, Massachusetts . It is currently available in a reprint edition (1994) or . (The 1989 edition did not include ''Merriam-'' ...
'' also follows. Richard Hudson and Mariangela Spinillo also categorize these words as pronouns but do not assume an appositive relationship between the pronoun and the rest of the noun phrase. However, two other features make these words resemble determiners rather than nouns. First, their phrase-initial position (''we students'') is typical of determiners (''the students''). Second, they cannot combine with other determiners (*''the we students''), which suggests that they fill the same role. These characteristics have led linguists like Ray Jackendoff and Steven Paul Abney to categorize such uses of ''we'' and ''you'' as determiners. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language'' similarly classifies this use of ''we'' and ''you'' as "an extended, secondary use" in which words that began as pronouns have been reanalyzed as determiners.Huddleston, Rodney, and Geoffrey K. Pullum. ''The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language''. Cambridge UP, 2002. pp. 421–422.


References and notes


Notes


References

{{reflist English grammar English words