Background
Varieties of ellipsis have long formed a basis ofTypes
There are numerous widely acknowledged types of ellipsis. They include, as mentioned and briefly illustrated below: # Gapping # Stripping # Verb phrase ellipsis # Pseudogapping # Answer ellipsis # Sluicing # Nominal ellipsis # Comparative deletion # Null complement anaphora Among experts, there is no unanimity that all of the abovementioned syntaxes form a natural class in the sense of being derived by one and the same mechanism. Ellipsis-based accounts have been given for other syntaxes, and some of the above have been analyzed in other ways. Most experts would agree, however, that most of the above items are in fact ellipses, so the discussion below takes their status as ellipses largely for granted. The example sentences below employ the convention whereby the elided material is indicated with subscripts and smaller font size. All examples given below come from English though similar patterns arise cross-linguistically, with variation from language to language.Gapping
Gapping occurs in coordinate structures. Redundant material that is present in the immediately preceding clause can be "gapped". This gapped material usually contains a finite verb. Canonical cases have a true "gap" insofar as a remnant appears to the left and to the right of the elided material. ::John can play the guitar, and Mary can play the violin. ::Fred took a picture of you, and Susan took a picture of me. While canonical cases have medial gaps as in these two sentences, the gap need not be medial, and it can even be discontinuous, e.g.: ::She persuaded him to do the homework, and he persuaded her to do the homework. ::Should I call you, or should you call me? While these two sentences again each have two remnants, the gapped material is no longer continuous. There are in a sense two gaps in each of the gapped clauses. Gapping has been thoroughly studied, and it is therefore reasonably well understood, although the theoretical analyses can vary significantly.Stripping
Stripping is also known as ''bare argument ellipsis''. Many linguists take stripping to be a particular manifestation of gapping whereby just one remnant appears in the gapped clause instead of the two (or more) that occur in instances of gapping. The fact that stripping is limited to occurring in coordinate structures is the main reason why stripping is integrated into the analysis of gapping: ::John can play the guitar, and Mary can play the guitar, too. ::Sam has attempted problem 1 twice, and he has attempted problem 2 also. These examples illustrate that stripping is flexible insofar as the remnant in the stripped clause is not limited in function; it can, for instance, be a subject as in the first sentence or an object as in the second sentence. A particularly frequent type of stripping is ''not''-stripping (stripping in the presence of ''not''), e.g.: ::Sam did it, not Fred did it. - ''not''-Stripping ::Sally is working on Monday,she is not working on Tuesday. ''Not''-stripping's status as a form of ellipsis can be debated, since the non-elliptical versions of these sentences are unacceptable. The key trait of ellipsis, namely, is that both versions are supposed to be acceptable (the elliptical and non-elliptical version).Verb phrase ellipsis
Verb phrase ellipsis (also VP-ellipsis or VPE) is a particularly frequent form of ellipsis in English. VP-ellipsis elides a non-finite VP. The ellipsis must be introduced by an auxiliary verb or by the particle ''to''. ::John can play the guitar; Mary can play the guitar, too. ::He has done it before, which means he will do it again. An aspect of VP-ellipsis that is unlike gapping and stripping is that it can occur forwards or backwards. That is, the ellipsis can precede or follow its antecedent, e.g.: ::The man who wanted to order the salmon did order the salmon. ::The man who wanted to order the salmon did order the salmon. Of the various ellipsis mechanisms, VP-ellipsis has probably been studied the most and it is therefore relatively well understood.Pseudogapping
Many linguists take pseudogapping to be a particular manifestation of VP-ellipsis (not of gapping). Like VP-ellipsis, pseudogapping is introduced by an auxiliary verb. Pseudogapping differs from VP-ellipsis, however, insofar as the elided VP is not entirely gone, but rather one (or more) remnants of the VP appear. This aspect of pseudogapping gives it the outward appearance of gapping. Pseudogapping occurs frequently in comparative and contrastive contexts: ::They have been eating the apples more than they have been eating the oranges. ::I will feed the chickens today if you will feed the chickens tomorrow. Pseudogapping is more restricted in distribution than VP-ellipsis. For instance it can hardly occur backwards, i.e. the ellipsis can hardly precede its antecedent. Further examples: ::Would you want to say that to me, or would I want to say that to you? ::They could read this book more easily than they could read that book. Another noteworthy trait of pseudogapping (and one that supports the view that it is a type of VP-ellipsis) is that it is absent from languages related to English.Answer ellipsis
Answer ellipsis involves question-answer pairs. The question focuses an unknown piece of information, often using anSluicing
Sluicing usually elides everything from a direct or indirect question except the question word. It is a frequent type of ellipsis that appears to occur in most if not all languages. It can operate both forwards and backwards like VP-ellipsis, but unlike gapping, stripping, answer fragments, and pseudogapping, e.g.: ::John can play something, but I don’t know what he can play. ::When he will call I don't know, but John will definitely call. The sluicing illustrated with these two sentences has occurred in indirect questions. Sluicing in direct questions is illustrated with the following two examples: ::A: Something unusual happened. B: What happened? ::A: He has been working on the problem. B: How long has he been working on the problem? Sluicing has been studied intensely in the past decade and can be viewed as a relatively well understood ellipsis mechanism, although the theoretical analysis of certain aspects of sluicing remains controversial.Nominal ellipsis
Noun ellipsis (also N-ellipsis, N'-ellipsis, NP-ellipsis, NPE, ellipsis in the DP) occurs when the noun and potentially accompanying modifiers are omitted from a noun phrase. Nominal ellipsis occurs with a limited set of determinatives in English (cardinal and ordinal numbers and possessive determiners), whereas it is much freer in other languages. The following examples illustrate nominal ellipsis with cardinal and ordinal numbers: ::Fred did three onerous tasks because Susan had done two onerous tasks. ::The first train and the second train have arrived. The following two sentences illustrate nominal ellipsis with possessive determiners: ::I heard Mary's dog, and you heard Bill's dog. ::If Doris tries my chili, I will try hers (her chili).Comparative deletion
Comparative deletion occurs in clauses introduced by ''than'' in English. The expression that is elided corresponds to a comparative morph such as ''more'' or ''-er'' in the antecedent clause, e.g.: ::More people arrived than we expected people would arrive. ::She ordered more beer than we could drink beer. ::Doris looks more satisfied than Doreen looks satisfied. ::William has friends in more countries than you have friends in countries. Comparative deletion is different from many of the other optional ellipsis mechanisms insofar as it is obligatory. The non-elliptical versions of these sentences are unacceptable.Null complement anaphora
Null complement anaphora elides a complete complement, whereby the elided complement is a finite clause, infinitive phrase, or prepositional phrase. The verbal predicates that can license null complement anaphora form a limited set (e.g. ''know'', ''approve'', ''refuse'', ''decide''). The elided complement cannot be a noun phrase. ::Q: Do you know what happened? A: No, I don't know what happened. ::Q: Do you approve of the plan? A: No, I don't approve of the plan. ::They told Bill to help, but he refused to help. ::They offered two ways to spend the day, but I couldn't decide between them. Of the various ellipsis mechanisms, null complement anaphora is the least studied. In this regard, its status as ellipsis is a point of debate, since its behavior is not consistent with the behavior of many of the other ellipsis mechanisms.Less-studied cases of ellipsis
Further instances of ellipsis that do not (in a clear way) qualify as any of the ellipsis types listed above: ::A: The cat likes Bill. B: Why does the cat articularlylike Bill? ::What will happen if I miss the deadline? More work on ellipsis may need to be done before all ellipsis mechanisms are fully explained.Theoretical approaches
Ellipsis is widely studied in the theoretical literature, central issues including the mental representation of elided material, the conditions which license ellipsis, and the means by which the elided material is recovered. One challenge to theoretical accounts of ellipsis comes from cases where the elided material does not appear to be aSee also
* Anaphora (linguistics) * Aposiopesis * Squiggle operator * Right node raising * Question under discussionReferences
Notes Bibliography *Ágel, V., Ludwig Eichinger, Hans-Werner Eroms, Peter Hellwig, Hans Heringer, and Hennig Lobin (eds.) 2003/6. Dependency and Valency: An international handbook of contemporary research. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. *Johnson, Kyle 2001. What VP ellipsis can do, and what it can't, but not why. In The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory, ed. Mark Baltin and Chris Collins, 439–479. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. *Lappin, Shalom 1996. The interpretation of ellipsis. In The handbook of contemporary semantic theory, ed. Shalom Lappin. Oxford: Blackwell. *Lobeck, Anne. 1995. Ellipsis: Functional heads, licensing, and identification. New York: Oxford University Press. *Lobeck, Anne. 2006. Ellipsis in DP. In The Blackwell Companion to Syntax, ed. by Martin Everaert et al., vol. 2, pp. 145–173. Oxford: Blackwell. *Merchant, Jason. 2001. The syntax of silence: Sluicing, islands, and the theory of ellipsis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. *Osborne, Timothy and Thomas Groß 2012. Constructions are catenae: Construction Grammar meets Dependency Grammar. Cognitive Linguistics 23, 1: 163–214. *Sag, Ivan 1976. Deletion and logical form. Doctoral Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts. {{Formal semantics Syntactic relationships Generative syntax Syntax Formal semantics (natural language) Grammar