Language and speakers
Marra is a member of the Arnhem family, the second-largest Australian language family after Pama–Nyungan.Bowern & Koch, 2004. ''Australian Languages: Classification and the Comparative Method'', p 44 The Marra people refer to themselves as or , and their language as . In addition to Warndarrang, which was spoken to the north of Marra along the Roper River, Marra was also in contact with Alawa (spoken inland, to the west), Binbinga and Wilangarra ( West Barkly languages to the south), and Yanyuwa (a Pama–Nyungan language to the southeast). The Marra people were traditionally divided into three clans that lived along the Limmen Bight River in Arnhem Land (Marra grammar
''(All grammatical information from Heath 1981 unless otherwise noted.)''Phonetics
Consonant inventory
Marra has a consonant inventory nearly identical to those of Warndarrang and Alawa. There are two additional phonemes: the interdental and which occur only in a few flora-fauna terms, and are likely loanwords from either Nunggubuyu or Yanyuwa, both of which languages use these phonemes frequently. With the interdentals excepted, the Marra consonants consist of a stop and a nasal in each of five places of articulation with two laterals, two rhotics, and twoVowel inventory
Marra has three main vowels: The vowel is found in exactly two words, , "paper wasp", and , "sandfly", and the vowel in one word, , a common interjection meaning "yes!" found throughout the area, including in the local English-based creole. There is no contrast in MarraPhonology
Clusters
Vowels clusters do not occur; all but one of adjacent underlying vowels are deleted. The only permitted word-initial consonant clusters are homorganic (involving the same place of articulation) nasal + stop combinations, particularly ''mb'' or ''ngg''. The nominative prefix ''n-'', when added to a stem beginning with a cluster, is usually pronounced with the preceding syllable, and the ''n-'' with combined with /r/ or /n/ results in the addition of the meaningless particle ''–nga-'' between the prefix and the stem. Word-final consonant clusters can only take the formLenition
In segments that are repeated in a word – either by reduplication or by chance morphology – the second stop is often lenited into a semivowel or lost altogether. /j/ and /ʈ/ will become /y/, /b/ will become /w/, and /g/ will either become /w/ or Ø. This lenition can optionally occur at the beginning of a small number of nouns when the stem is preceded by a prefix ending in a vowel. There are also several instances of word-initial lenition of /g/ or /b/ to /w/, in cardinal directions, kin terms, and a few other isolated examples. At the beginning of verb stems, the underlying combination ''rrn'' will have the surface form of n, whereas an n followed by the phonemes l, rl, rr, r, n, or ny in any other context results in the deletion of the initial n.Nasalization
Stops are frequently nasalized (pronounced as the nasal at the stop place of articulation) when followed by a nasal or any other non-stop. Examples of this include the reduplicated "to mix a lot" from "to mix" or the noun + case ending of from " juvenile euro (Macropus robustus)".Nominal morphology
In Marra, there is no clear grammatical distinction between nouns, adjectives, and adverbs; they are all treated the same morphologically. Personal and demonstrative pronouns, however, each form a distinctive word class, and all can be clearly distinguished from verb complexes.Articles
Nouns are usually preceded by an article, which marks case, gender, and number. The nominative articles, for instance, are as follows:Case prefixes
In additional to the articles, each noun is marked with a prefix containing information about case ( nominative or non-nominative), gender (masculine, feminine, or neuter), and number (singular, plural, dual), as follows: Almost all non-human singular nouns are marked as masculine, though some specifically-female marsupial terms can be marked as feminine. The neuter case is reserved for body parts, topographic terms, abstract conceptions, and the word , "sun".Case suffixes
Nouns in Marra are marked by suffixes for one of six cases: # nominative # ergative/Possession
Possession is typically marked by a genitive pronoun, though if the possessor noun (in the ergative/instrumental case) is present the pronoun is sometimes omitted. For example, means "his camp" with the third person singular genitive pronoun , and either or can mean "the man camp."Quantifiers
Marra has five basic numerals, one through five: The numerals six through ten are expressed by combining "five" with another number, e.g., for "nine". There are also more general quantifiers such as and , "many"; , "big group" (non-human); , "big group" (human); and , "a few".Reduplication
Like many Australian languages, Marra has a process known as reduplication, where some or all of a stem is repeated. With human nouns, reduplication takes the meaning of three or more of that noun, such as , "three or more old people" from , "old person", and a few topographic nouns can be reduplicated to mean the collective plural, as in , "islands". With both human and non-human nouns, reduplication along with the pergressive case suffix can create the meaning "having X" or "having lots of X", as in , "having a woman" (being a married man) from , "woman". A few verb stems also display partial reduplication to indicate a repeated action, as in , "he repeatedly tied it or them up" as opposed to , "he was tying it or them up".Personal pronouns
In addition to the pronoun markers on nouns (see above) and verbs (see below), Marra also has independent personal pronouns. Unlike other nouns, pronouns do not show a nominative/ergative distinction but instead use the nominative form to mark all subjects as well as the direct object of a transitive verb. Because these pronouns are marked within the verb clause, their inclusion is often optional and can be used to highlight a particular point in what is known as the "emphatic" case. Personal pronouns have paradigms in seven cases – nominative, emphatic, genitive, ablative, oblique stem, allative/ locative, and purposive – for each of first person (singular, exclusive dual, inclusive dual, exclusive plural, and inclusive plural), second person (singular, dual, and plural), and third person (masculine singular, feminine singular, neuter singular, dual, and plural).Demonstrative pronouns
There are five categories for demonstrative pronouns: proximate, localized immediate, unlocalized immediate, distant, and anaphoric. With the proximate stems, there are separate forms for predicative (in the "predicate" of the sentence, or the part that modifies the subject) or nonpredicative nouns. Proximate refers to the area around the speaker, the equivalent of "here". The immediate refers to the area around the person being addressed or to the area approximately two meters away from the speaker. The localized immediate specifies the location, whereas the unlocalized immediate, which is rarer, is more general. The distant category refers to anything outside of the immediate, either visible to the speaker or invisible. The anaphoric category is anything within the distant category that has previously been referred to, indicating that the location is not new to the discourse. These pronouns have separate forms for masculine singular, feminine singular, neuter, dual, and plural, each of which has a nominative and non-nominative form. They are generally formed by the prefixes , , , or , and or for the nominative or , , , , and for the non-nominative and the suffixes (non-predicative proximate), (predicative proximate), (unlocalized immediate), (localized immediate), or (distant), and or (anaphoric), though there are irregular forms for some combinations. From these, one can form demonstrative adverbs, in the locative or allative cases. These have the same spatial meaning as the corresponding demonstrative pronouns, but they refer to a general location rather than the location of a specific noun. The allative forms are summarized in the following table: To make the locative forms, the in the table above is replaced by the prefix (proximate), (immediate), or (distant or anaphoric), and the suffix is added.Cardinal directions
Like many of the languages of Arnhem Land, Marra cardinal directions correspond closely with English "north, south, east, west", but have intricate case morphology. There are also directional words for "up" and "down" (i.e., upriver, downhill, etc.) that display a similar morphological complexity:Interrogation
Yes–no questions in Marra are identical to assertions, with a slight intonation difference. There is no tag for these statements (an equivalent to the English "right?" or "arent you?"), though the local English-based creoles question marker occasionally appears in modern Marra speech. Other types of interrogative clauses involve words that can also take an indefinite form, as in , which can mean "who?", "someone", or "anyone". If the distinction between interrogative and indefinite is unclear from context, the adverb "maybe" can be added to indicate that the phrase is an assertion and not a question. These interrogative words take a prefix to mark number and gender – masculine singular is the default, though any additional presupposed information can be included in the marking. Case suffixes can also be marked. These particles are typically clause-initial and then followed by the assertion whose details are being elicited. For instance, literally means "for what? he went" with the sense of "why did he go?" and means "what? you killed it" or "what did you kill?".Verbal morphology
A basic verb complex in Marra consists of a pronominal prefix, an inflectable verb-stem, and suffixes marking tense, aspect, and mood. Often, however, there is an uninflectable "main verb" that specifies the meaning of the verb that is then followed by the inflectable "auxiliary verb". Some verbs in Marra can only be main verbs or auxiliary verbs, though many can serve in both positions.Order of the verb complex
The morphemes of the verb are ordered in the complex as follows: # Negative (prefix or preceding word or ) # Benefactive or # Main verb # Centripetal or # Third person present marker # Pronoun prefix # Reduplication of any prior prefixes # Durative stem-initial prefix # Auxiliary verb # Tense, aspect, mood suffix # Reflexive/reciprocal suffix The benefactive prefix indicates that something was done "for" somebody as, as in , "he killed it for me". is used when there is a main-auxiliary distinction; is used when there is only one verb in the complex. The centripetal particle is used to indicate motion within the speaker’s frame of reference, with the idea of the motion coming "back" or "This way". It is the only way to distinguish the meaning of verbs "to take" from "to bring" or "to go" from "to come".Inflectional categories
Marra has sixteen possible inflectional (tense/aspect/mood) categories: * Past punctual positive * Past continuous durative positive * Past continuous nondurative positive * Past potential positive * Past potential negative * Past negative * Present negative * Present positive * Evitative positive * Future indefinite positive * Future punctual positive * Future continuous durative positive * Future continuous nondurative positive * Imperative positive * Desiderative positive * Future negative Marra aspect is split between continuous and punctual (also known as " perfective") actions, with the former divided into durative (happened throughout) and non-durative (happened over time, but not the entire time). The positive/negative division distinguishes things that did, are, or will happen from things that did not, are not, or will not happen, a category termed in the analyses of some neighboring languages as " irrealis". The "future indefinite" category is quite rare and takes the meaning of "might". The "past potential" refers to something that was just about to happen (but didn't, due to an interruption) or should have happened. The evitative category might be translated as "lest" or "or else", indicating that something undesirable might occur if something else is or is not done. For example, means "give it to me, or else I will hit you". Note that the evitative is normally paired with another clause (as Heath says, it “does not normally stand alone as a simple prediction of impending doom”), usually in the imperative. The past continuous durative positive, present negative/positive, future indefinite positive, future continuous durative positive, and desiderative positive all take a "durative" morpheme in the verb complex's "durative stem-initial prefix" slot; all other categories are unmarked. The forms of these suffixes differ by auxiliary verb.Pronominal prefixes
As in Warndarrang and other related languages, a different pronominal prefix is added to the verb for each combination of subject and object. For example, a verb with a second-person singular subject and a first-person exclusive dual object would take the prefix but the person-person exclusive dual subject with a third-person dual object would take the prefix . Within the second-person subject, third-person object paradigm, there are also different pronouns for imperative and non-imperative verbs. There are extremely complicated rules, with many exceptions, for generating these pronouns. When the third person or third person subject/third person object category is marked, the additional prefix is added to the complex.Syntax
Word order
Within a noun phrase (NP) or verb complex, word order is almost completely fixed. Articles are followed by demonstrative pronouns are followed by the main noun are followed by adjectives, though genitive pronouns may either follow or precede the main noun. For verbs, the negative particles must immediately precede the verb complex, and within the complex the order of the morphemes is strictly set. Within the clause, however, the order of the NPs, verb complexes, and adverbs is free. The first element is typically considered to be the most important element. If the first element is not a verb complex, the main verb complex commonly but not always assumes the second position; there appears to be no difference in meaning between those sentences that place the verb complex in the second position and those that do not.Subordinated clauses
Subordinated clauses are typically marked by a particle or conjunction such as , "because" or , "after that". If, however, the clause can be reduced to a single verb complex, that word is typically nominalized using the suffixes or and then placed following the head noun.Avoidance terminology
Marra, like many languages of the area, has taboos preventing the direct interaction of siblings of the opposite sex, beginning around age eight (the age of circumcision in males). The only specific avoidance term in Marra, however, is , used by a sister of a boy who has been circumcised to address or refer to him. In any other situation, the term for a circumcised boy is . Marra does not have the complex avoidance speech or male-female language distinction that is found in neighboring Yanyula. Men are, however, not supposed to pronounce the names of their mother-in-law (wife’s mother), their wife mother brother, or their wife brother, though these taboos are relaxed as a man ages.Comparison of the Marran languages
Warndarrang (a language not spoken since 1974) and Marra (a language with only a small number of speakers) are each other’s closest relatives. Together with Alawa (also critically endangered) and Yugul (a language attested by speakers of Warndarrang, Marra, and Alawa but apparently extinct, these languages form the Marran subgroup of the Gunwinyguan language family. The three documented languages share much vocabulary and have many similar grammatical structures, though there are significant differences, and Warndarrang has been heavily influenced by loanwords from Nunggubuyu and Ngandi to the north.Verbal comparison
All three languages are prefixing, and their verbs consist of either a single inflected stem or an uninflected "main verb" preceding an inflected auxiliary verb. Such verbal particles are absent in the languages to the north. The Marran languages also share verbal features such as particle reduplication within the verbal complex indicating a repeated or continuous action (a pattern common in Australian languages), and the negation of verbs is indicated by a particle immediately preceding the verb complex ( in both Warndarang and Marra but in Alawa). Marra has a significantly more complex verbal inflection system than Warndarrang (sixteen different tense/aspect/mood categories in Marra but only eight in Warndarrang and apparently seven in Alawa), an unusually intricate system for Australian languages. Both languages, however, have conjugation paradigms that are highly verb-specific. In addition to the similarities in the order of the verb complex, Marra and Warndarrang also both use word-order to focus, or highlight, a particular item within the clause, though otherwise the word-order in Marra is far stricter than that in Warndarrang.Nominal comparison
Alawa divides its nouns into two genders (masculine and feminine) while Marra has three classes (masculine, feminine, and neuter) and Warndarrang six. All three languages distinguish between singular, dual, and plural, with Warndarrang having an additional "paucal" (three to five) class for human nouns. The use of noun cases in Warndarrang and Marra are nearly identical – Marra condenses the allative and locative cases and adds a pergressive case – though the only cognate across the paradigm is the purposive . The case marking system of Alawa is apparently not related. The demonstratives in Warndarrang and Marra cover approximately the same semantic categories (proximate, immediate, distant, and anaphoric, though Warndarrang adds an intermediate near-distant), though the forms themselves have little similarity. In fact, the Marra demonstratives inflect for case, number, and gender, while Warndarrang demonstratives engage a single basic form. Again, the Alawa demonstrative system is entirely separate, drawing only a single distance distinction ("this" versus "that") but with more nuanced anaphoric distinctions. The directional terminology between Warndarang and Marra shares many cognates, such as (Marra) and (Warndarrang) for "west" or (both languages) for "north", though Marra again has a far more intricate and irregular morphological system to distinguish cases in these terms. Marra also has an up/down directional distinction that is absent in Warndarrang. There is no Alawa data for cardinal directions.Lexical comparison
Cultural terminology between the three languages is distinct. Marra has an extremely complex kinship terminology system, including a large number of dyadic terms; Warndarrang’s system appeared to be much simpler, though the linguist Jeffrey Heath was unable to elicit much kinship information before his informant died. Alawa has a morphologically-irregular system similar to Marra, but lacks the dyadic terms and shares few cognates (exceptions include for "older sibling"). A cursory analysis of the flora-fauna terms in the three languages also reveals few cognates. The semi-moieties in Warndarrang and Marra have nearly identical names, however, though the groups were associated with different totems, songs, and rituals.See also
* Non-Pama–Nyungan languages *Notes
References
* * * * * * * {{DEFAULTSORT:Mara Language (Australia) Endangered indigenous Australian languages in the Northern Territory Mangarrayi–Maran languages