
Automaticity is the ability to do things without occupying the
mind
The mind is the set of faculties responsible for all mental phenomena. Often the term is also identified with the phenomena themselves. These faculties include thought, imagination, memory, will, and sensation. They are responsible for vario ...
with the low-level details required, allowing it to become an automatic response pattern or
habit. It is usually the result of
learning,
repetition
Repetition may refer to:
*Repetition (rhetorical device), repeating a word within a short space of words
* Repetition (bodybuilding), a single cycle of lifting and lowering a weight in strength training
*Working title for the 1985 slasher film '' ...
, and practice. Examples of tasks carried out by '
muscle memory
Muscle memory is a form of procedural memory that involves consolidating a specific motor task into memory through repetition, which has been used synonymously with motor learning. When a movement is repeated over time, the brain creates a long- ...
' often involve some degree of automaticity.
Examples of automaticity are common activities such as walking, speaking, bicycle-riding, assembly-line work, and driving a car (the last of these sometimes being termed "
highway hypnosis"). After an activity is sufficiently practiced, it is possible to focus the mind on other activities or thoughts while undertaking an automatized activity (for example, holding a conversation or planning a speech while driving a car).
Characteristics
John Bargh (1994), based on over a decade of research, suggested that four characteristics usually accompany automatic behavior:
;Awareness
:A person may be unaware of the mental process that is occurring.
;Intentionality
:A person may not be involved with the initiation of a mental process.
;Efficiency
:Automatic mental processes tend to have a low
cognitive load, requiring relatively low mental resources.
;Controllability
:A person may not have the ability to stop or alter a process after initiation.
Bargh states that these are simply common characteristics; not all are needed for a process to be considered automatic. For instance,
stereotype activation has been described as an automatic process: it is unintentional and efficient, requiring little effort. However stereotype activation is accompanied by above chance awareness and if conflicting processing goals are available then it becomes controlled. Therefore, stereotype activation only satisfies two of Bargh's criteria, but is still considered to be an example of automatic processing.
In reading
LaBerge and Samuels (1974) helped explain how reading fluency develops. Automaticity refers to knowing how to perform some arbitrary task at a competent level without requiring conscious effort — ''i.e.'', it is a form of
unconscious competence.
Moreover, if the student is automatic or is "a skilled reader, multiple tasks are being performed at the same time, such as decoding the words, comprehending the information, relating the information to prior knowledge of the subject matter, making inferences, and evaluating the information's usefulness to a report he or she is writing" (Samuels). It is essential to understand automaticity and how it is achieved to better a student's performance. This is important for teachers because automaticity should be focused on in early years to ensure higher level reading skills in adolescence.
Disruption
Automaticity can be disrupted by explicit
attention
Attention is the behavioral and cognitive process of selectively concentrating on a discrete aspect of information, whether considered subjective or objective, while ignoring other perceivable information. William James (1890) wrote that "Att ...
when the devotion of conscious attention to the pattern alters the content or timing of that pattern itself. This phenomenon is especially pronounced in situations that feature high upside and/or downside risk and impose the associated psychological stress on one's conscious mind; one's performance in these situations may either ''a'') be unimpaired or even enhanced ("
flow
Flow may refer to:
Science and technology
* Fluid flow, the motion of a gas or liquid
* Flow (geomorphology), a type of mass wasting or slope movement in geomorphology
* Flow (mathematics), a group action of the real numbers on a set
* Flow (psych ...
") or ''b'') deteriorate ("
choke").
This effect has been named the "centipede effect" after the fable of the "
Centipede's dilemma", where a toad immobilises a centipede simply by asking it how it walks. The centipede's normally unconscious locomotion was interrupted by conscious reflection on it. The psychologist George Humphrey referred to this parable in his 1923 ''The story of man's mind'':
"No man skilled at a trade needs to put his constant attention on the routine work," he wrote. "If he does, the job is apt to be spoiled."
Use to influence
In ''Influence'',
Robert Cialdini's book about
social psychology
Social psychology is the scientific study of how thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are influenced by the real or imagined presence of other people or by social norms. Social psychologists typically explain human behavior as a result of the ...
and influence tactics, Cialdini explains how common
automatic response patterns
Automatic may refer to:
Music Bands
* Automatic (band), Australian rock band
* Automatic (American band), American rock band
* The Automatic, a Welsh alternative rock band
Albums
* ''Automatic'' (Jack Bruce album), a 1983 electronic rock ...
are in
human behavior
Human behavior is the potential and expressed capacity ( mentally, physically, and socially) of human individuals or groups to respond to internal and external stimuli throughout their life. Kagan, Jerome, Marc H. Bornstein, and Richard ...
, and how easily they can be triggered, even with erroneous cues. He describes an experiment conducted by social psychologists
Langer
Langer is a surname originally of German origin. For the etymology, meaning, and pronunciation of the name, and for the Hiberno-English slang word, see Wiktionary.
People with the family name Langer include:
Academics and scientists
* Alois Lang ...
, Chanowitz, and Blank which illustrates how compliant people will be with a request if they hear words that sound like they are being given a reason, even if no actual reason is provided. The experimenters approached people standing in line to use a photocopier with one of three requests:
* "Excuse me. I have 5 pages. May I use the Xerox machine because I'm in a rush?"
* "Excuse me. I have 5 pages. May I use the Xerox machine?"
* "Excuse me. I have 5 pages. May I use the Xerox machine because I have to make some copies?"
When given the request plus a reason, 94% of people asked complied with the request. When given the request without a reason, only 60% complied. But when given the request with what sounds like a reason but isn't, compliance jumped back to 93%. Langer, Chanowitz, and Blank are convinced that most human behavior falls into automatic response patterns.
However, when the request was made larger (20 pages instead of 5), subjects expected a sound reason before complying, as illustrated in the table.
See also
*
Habit (psychology)
A habit (or wont as a humorous and formal term) is a routine of behavior that is repeated regularly and tends to occur subconsciously.
*
Habituation
Habituation is a form of non-associative learning in which an innate (non-reinforced) response to a stimulus decreases after repeated or prolonged presentations of that stimulus. Responses that habituate include those that involve the intact org ...
*
Neural adaptation
*
Implicit memory
*
Unconscious cognition
*
Unconscious thought theory Unconscious thought theory (UTT) posits that the unconscious mind is capable of performing tasks outside of one's awareness, and that unconscious thought (UT) is better at solving complex tasks, where many variables are considered, than conscious th ...
References
Sources
* PhysioEx 6.0 – Peter Zao – Timothy Stabler – Greta Peterson – Lori Smith
*
* {{cite journal , last1 = Samuels , first1 = S. J. , last2 = Flor , first2 = R.F. , year = 1997 , title = The Importance of Automaticity for Developing Expertise in Reading , journal = Reading & Writing Quarterly , volume = 13 , issue = 2, pages = 107–121 , doi=10.1080/1057356970130202
* Cialdini, R.B. (2001). Influence: Science and practice (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. p. 4
External links
Automaticity at Encyclopedia of Educational TechnologyThis online .pdf summarizes the research about automatic processing and controlled processing up to 2003
Cognitive science
Learning
Educational technology