Colors in eV.svg
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Color (American English) or colour (British English) is the visual perception, visual perceptual Physical property, property deriving from the spectrum of light interacting with the photoreceptor cells of the eyes. Color categories and physical specifications of color are associated with objects or materials based on their physical properties such as light absorption, reflection, or emission spectra. By defining a color space, colors can be identified numerically by their coordinates. Because perception of color stems from the varying spectral sensitivity of different types of cone cells in the retina to different parts of the spectrum, colors may be defined and quantified by the degree to which they stimulate these cells. These physical or physiological quantifications of color, however, do not fully explain the psychophysics, psychophysical perception of color appearance. Color science includes the color vision, perception of color by the human eye, eye and brain, the origin of color in materials, color theory in art, and the physics of electromagnetic radiation in the visible range (i.e. ''light'').


Physics of color

Electromagnetic radiation is characterized by its wavelength (or frequency) and its luminous intensity, intensity. When the wavelength is within the visible spectrum (the range of wavelengths humans can perceive, approximately from 390 nanometre, nm to 700 nm), it is known as "visible light". Most light sources emit light at many different wavelengths; a source's ''spectrum'' is a distribution giving its intensity at each wavelength. Although the spectrum of light arriving at the eye from a given direction determines the color wikt:sensation, sensation in that direction, there are many more possible spectral combinations than color sensations. In fact, one may formally define a color as a class of spectra that give rise to the same color sensation, although such classes would vary widely among different species, and to a lesser extent among individuals within the same species. In each such class, the members are called ''metamerism (color), metamers'' of the color in question. This effect can be visualized by comparing the light sources' spectral power distributions and the resulting colors.


Spectral colors

The familiar colors of the rainbow in the visible spectrum, spectrum—named using the Latin word for ''appearance'' or ''apparition'' by Isaac Newton in 1671—include all those colors that can be produced by visible light of a single wavelength only, the spectral color, ''pure spectral'' or ''monochromatic'' colors. The table at right shows approximate frequencies (in hertz, terahertz) and wavelengths (in nanometre, nanometers) for spectral colors in the visible range. Spectral colors have 100% Colorfulness#Excitation purity, purity, and are fully colorfulness, saturated. A complex mixture of spectral colors can be used to describe any color, which is the definition of a light power spectrum. The color table should not be interpreted as a definitive list; the spectral colors form a continuous spectrum, and how it is divided into color term, distinct colors linguistically is a matter of culture and historical contingency. Despite the ubiquitous ROYGBIV mnemonic used to remember the spectral colors in english, the inclusion or exclusion of colors in this table is contentious, with disagreement often focused on Indigo#Classification as a spectral color, indigo and cyan. Even if the subset of color terms is agreed, their wavelength ranges and borders between them may not be. The ''intensity'' of a spectral color, relative to the context in which it is viewed, may alter its perception considerably according to the Bezold–Brücke shift; for example, a low-intensity orange-yellow is brown, and a low-intensity yellow-green is olive (color)#Olive green, olive green.


Color of objects

The color of an object as perceived by an observer is not an intrinsic quality of that object, but depends on several factors: # the physics of the object (which wavelengths of light are selectively absorbed, reflected, transmitted, or emitted) # the color of the light shining on the object (color cast of the illuminant) # the angles between observer, object and illuminant (applicable to structural color) # the physics of light in its environment (how the atmosphere may affect the light through Rayleigh scattering or Dispersion (optics), dispersion, for example) # relative velocity between object and observer (red shift; mostly applicable to astronomy) # the characteristics of the perceiving eye (the number and spectral sensitivity of cone cell, cone classes and Color vision#dimensionality, dimensionality of color vision) # higher order processes in the brain that affect the color, such as color constancy Some generalizations of the physics can be drawn, neglecting perceptual effects for now: *Light arriving at an opacity (optics), opaque surface is either reflection (physics), reflected "specular reflection, specularly" (that is, in the manner of a mirror), scattering, scattered (that is, reflected with diffuse scattering), or absorption (electromagnetic radiation), absorbed—or some combination of these. *Opaque objects that do not reflect specularly (which tend to have rough surfaces) have their color determined by which wavelengths of light they scatter strongly (with the light that is not scattered being absorbed). If objects scatter all wavelengths with roughly equal strength, they appear white. If they absorb all wavelengths, they appear black. *Opaque objects that specularly reflect the light of different wavelengths with different efficiencies look like mirrors tinted with colors determined by those differences. An object that reflects some fraction of impinging light and absorbs the rest may look black but also be faintly reflective; examples are black objects coated with layers of enamel or lacquer. *Objects that transparency and translucency, transmit light are either ''translucent'' (scattering the transmitted light) or ''transparent'' (not scattering the transmitted light). If they also absorb (or reflect) light of various wavelengths differentially, they appear tinted with a color determined by the nature of that absorption (or that reflectance). *Objects may emit light that they generate from having excited electrons, rather than merely reflecting or transmitting light. The electrons may be excited due to elevated temperature (''incandescence''), as a result of chemical reactions (''chemiluminescence''), after absorbing light of other frequencies ("fluorescence" or "phosphorescence") or from electrical contacts as in light-emitting diodes, or other list of light sources, light sources. To summarize, the color of an object is a complex result of its surface properties, its transmission properties, and its emission properties, all of which contribute to the mix of wavelengths in the light leaving the surface of the object. The perceived color is then further conditioned by the nature of the ambient illumination, and by the color properties of other objects nearby, and via other characteristics of the perceiving eye and brain.


Perception


Development of theories of color vision

Although Aristotle and other ancient scientists had already written on the nature of light and color vision, it was not until Isaac Newton, Newton that light was identified as the source of the color sensation. In 1810, Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, Goethe published his comprehensive ''Theory of Colours, Theory of Colors'' in which he provided a rational description of colour experience, which 'tells us how it originates, not what it is'. (Schopenhauer) In 1801 Thomas Young (scientist), Thomas Young proposed his trichromacy, trichromatic theory, based on the observation that any color could be matched with a combination of three lights. This theory was later refined by James Clerk Maxwell and Hermann von Helmholtz. As Helmholtz puts it, "the principles of Newton's law of mixture were experimentally confirmed by Maxwell in 1856. Young's theory of color sensations, like so much else that this marvelous investigator achieved in advance of his time, remained unnoticed until Maxwell directed attention to it." At the same time as Helmholtz, Ewald Hering developed the opponent process theory of color, noting that color blindness and afterimages typically come in opponent pairs (red-green, blue-orange, yellow-violet, and black-white). Ultimately these two theories were synthesized in 1957 by Hurvich and Jameson, who showed that retinal processing corresponds to the trichromatic theory, while processing at the level of the lateral geniculate nucleus corresponds to the opponent theory. In 1931, an international group of experts known as the ''Commission internationale de l'éclairage'' (International Commission on Illumination, CIE) developed a mathematical color model, which mapped out the space of observable colors and assigned a set of three numbers to each.


Color in the eye

The ability of the human eye to distinguish colors is based upon the varying sensitivity of different cells in the retina to light of different wavelengths. Humans are trichromacy, trichromatic—the retina contains three types of color receptor cells, or cone cell, cones. One type, relatively distinct from the other two, is most responsive to light that is perceived as blue or blue-violet, with wavelengths around 450 nanometre, nm; cones of this type are sometimes called ''short-wavelength cones'' or ''S cones'' (or misleadingly, ''blue cones''). The other two types are closely related genetically and chemically: ''middle-wavelength cones'', ''M cones'', or ''green cones'' are most sensitive to light perceived as green, with wavelengths around 540 nm, while the ''long-wavelength cones'', ''L cones'', or ''red cones'', are most sensitive to light that is perceived as greenish yellow, with wavelengths around 570 nm. Light, no matter how complex its composition of wavelengths, is reduced to three color components by the eye. Each cone type adheres to the principle of univariance, which is that each cone's output is determined by the amount of light that falls on it over all wavelengths. For each location in the visual field, the three types of cones yield three signals based on the extent to which each is stimulated. These amounts of stimulation are sometimes called ''tristimulus values''. The response curve as a function of wavelength varies for each type of cone. Because the curves overlap, some tristimulus values do not occur for any incoming light combination. For example, it is not possible to stimulate ''only'' the mid-wavelength (so-called "green") cones; the other cones will inevitably be stimulated to some degree at the same time. The set of all possible tristimulus values determines the human ''color space''. It has been estimated that humans can distinguish roughly 10 million different colors. The other type of light-sensitive cell in the eye, the rod cell, rod, has a different response curve. In normal situations, when light is bright enough to strongly stimulate the cones, rods play virtually no role in vision at all. On the other hand, in dim light, the cones are understimulated leaving only the signal from the rods, resulting in a black and white, colorless response. (Furthermore, the rods are barely sensitive to light in the "red" range.) In certain conditions of intermediate illumination, the rod response and a weak cone response can together result in color discriminations not accounted for by cone responses alone. These effects, combined, are summarized also in the Kruithof curve, which describes the change of color perception and pleasingness of light as a function of temperature and intensity.


Color in the brain

While the mechanisms of color vision at the level of the retina are well-described in terms of tristimulus values, color processing after that point is organized differently. A dominant theory of color vision proposes that color information is transmitted out of the eye by three opponent processes, or opponent channels, each constructed from the raw output of the cones: a red–green channel, a blue–yellow channel, and a black–white "luminance" channel. This theory has been supported by neurobiology, and accounts for the structure of our subjective color experience. Specifically, it explains why humans cannot perceive a "reddish green" or "yellowish blue", and it predicts the color wheel: it is the collection of colors for which at least one of the two color channels measures a value at one of its extremes. The exact nature of color perception beyond the processing already described, and indeed the status of color as a feature of the perceived world or rather as a feature of our ''perception'' of the world—a type of qualia—is a matter of complex and continuing philosophical dispute.


Nonstandard color perception


Color vision deficiency

A color vision deficiency causes an individual to perceive a smaller gamut of colors than the standard observer with normal color vision. The effect can be mild, having lower "color resolution" (i.e. anomalous trichromacy), moderate, lacking an entire dimension or channel of color (e.g. dichromacy), or complete, lacking all color perception (i.e. monochromacy). Most forms of color blindness derive from one or many of the three classes of cone cells either being missing, having a shifted spectral sensitivity or having lower responsiveness to incoming light. In addition, cerebral achromatopsia is caused by neural anomalies in those parts of the brain where visual processing takes place. Some colors that appear distinct to an individual with normal color vision will appear Metamerism (color), metameric to the color blind. The most common form of color blindness is congenital red-green color blindness, affecting ~8% of males. Individuals with the strongest form of this condition (dichromacy) will experience blue and purple, green and yellow, teal and gray as colors of confusion, i.e. metamers.


Tetrachromacy

Outside of humans, which are mostly ''trichromatic'' (having three types of cones), most mammals are dichromatic, possessing only two cones. However, outside of mammals, most vertebrate are ''tetrachromacy, tetrachromatic'', having four types of cones, and includes most, birds, reptiles, amphibians and teleost, bony fish. An extra dimension of color vision means these vertebrates can see two distinct colors that a normal human would view as Metamerism (color), metamers. Some invertebrates, such as the mantis shrimp, have an even higher number of cones (12) that could lead to a richer color gamut than even imaginable by humans. The existence of human tetrachromats is a contentious notion. As many as Tetrachromacy#Tetrachromacy in carriers of CVD, half of all human females have 4 distinct cone classes, which could enable tetrachromacy. However, a distinction must be made between ''retinal (or weak) tetrachromats'', which express four cone classes in the retina, and ''functional (or strong) tetrachromats'', which are able to make the enhanced color discriminations expected of tetrachromats. In fact, there is only one peer-reviewed report of a functional tetrachromat. It is estimated that while the average person is able to see one million colors, someone with functional tetrachromacy could see a hundred million colors.


Synesthesia

In certain forms of synesthesia, perceiving letters and numbers (grapheme–color synesthesia) or hearing sounds (chromesthesia) will evoke a perception of color. Behavioral and functional neuroimaging experiments have demonstrated that these color experiences lead to changes in behavioral tasks and lead to increased activation of brain regions involved in color perception, thus demonstrating their reality, and similarity to real color percepts, albeit evoked through a non-standard route. Synesthesia can occur genetically, with 4% of the population having variants associated with the condition. Synesthesia has also been known to occur with brain damage, drugs, and sensory deprivation. The philosopher Pythagoras experienced synesthesia and provided one of the first written accounts of the condition in approximately 550 BCE. He created mathematical equations for musical notes that could form part of a scale, such as an octave.


Afterimages

After exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range, photoreceptor cell, photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized. For a few seconds after the light ceases, they will continue to signal less strongly than they otherwise would. Colors observed during that period will appear to lack the color component detected by the desensitized photoreceptors. This effect is responsible for the phenomenon of afterimages, in which the eye may continue to see a bright figure after looking away from it, but in a complementary colors, complementary color. Afterimage effects have also been used by artists, including Vincent van Gogh.


Color constancy

When an artist uses a limited color scheme, color palette, the human eye tends to compensate by seeing any gray or neutral color as the color which is missing from the color wheel. For example, in a limited palette consisting of red, yellow, black, and white, a mixture of yellow and black will appear as a variety of green, a mixture of red and black will appear as a variety of purple, and pure gray will appear bluish. The trichromatic theory is strictly true when the visual system is in a fixed state of adaptation. In reality, the visual system is constantly adapting to changes in the environment and compares the various colors in a scene to reduce the effects of the illumination. If a scene is illuminated with one light, and then with another, as long as the difference between the light sources stays within a reasonable range, the colors in the scene appear relatively constant to us. This was studied by Edwin H. Land in the 1970s and led to his retinex theory of color constancy. Both phenomena are readily explained and mathematically modeled with modern theories of chromatic adaptation and color appearance (e.g. CIECAM02, iCAM).M.D. Fairchild
Color Appearance Models
, 2nd Ed., Wiley, Chichester (2005).
There is no need to dismiss the trichromatic theory of vision, but rather it can be enhanced with an understanding of how the visual system adapts to changes in the viewing environment.


Color naming

Colors vary in several different ways, including hue (shades of red, orange (colour), orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet (color), violet), colorfulness, saturation, brightness, and gloss (optics), gloss. Some color words are derived from the name of an object of that color, such as "orange (colour), orange" or "salmon (color), salmon", while others are abstract, like "red". In the 1969 study ''Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution'', Brent Berlin and Paul Kay describe a pattern in naming "basic" colors (like "red" but not "red-orange" or "dark red" or "blood red", which are "shades" of red). All languages that have two "basic" color names distinguish dark/cool colors from bright/warm colors. The next colors to be distinguished are usually red and then yellow or green. All languages with six "basic" colors include black, white, red, green, blue, and yellow. The pattern holds up to a set of twelve: black, gray, white, pink, red, orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, brown, and azure (color), azure (distinct from blue in Russian language, Russian and Italian language, Italian, but not English).


In culture

Colors, their meanings and associations can play a major role in works of art, including literature.


Associations

Individual colors have a variety of cultural associations such as national colours, national colors (in general described in individual color articles and color symbolism). The field of color psychology attempts to identify the effects of color on human emotion and activity. Chromotherapy is a form of alternative medicine attributed to various Eastern traditions. Colors have different associations in different countries and cultures. Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men. The combination of the colors red and yellow together can induce hunger, which has been capitalized on by a number of chain restaurants. Color plays a role in memory development too. A photograph that is in black and white is slightly less memorable than one in color. Studies also show that wearing bright colors makes you more memorable to people you meet.


Color reproduction

Color reproduction is the science of creating colors for the human eye that faithfully represent the desired color. It focuses on how to construct a spectrum of wavelengths that will best evoke a certain color in an observer. Most colors are not #Spectral colors, spectral colors, meaning they are mixtures of various wavelengths of light. However, these non-spectral colors are often described by their dominant wavelength, which identifies the single wavelength of light that produces a sensation most similar to the non-spectral color. Dominant wavelength is roughly akin to hue. There are many color perceptions that by definition cannot be pure spectral colors due to colorfulness, desaturation or because they are purples (mixtures of red and violet light, from opposite ends of the spectrum). Some examples of necessarily non-spectral colors are the achromatic colors (black, gray, and white) and colors such as pink, tan (color), tan, and magenta. Two different light spectra that have the same effect on the three color receptors in the human eye will be perceived as the same color. They are metamerism (color), metamers of that color. This is exemplified by the white light emitted by fluorescent lamps, which typically has a spectrum of a few narrow bands, while daylight has a continuous spectrum. The human eye cannot tell the difference between such light spectra just by looking into the light source, although the color rendering index of each light source may affect the color of objects illuminated by these metameric light sources. Similarly, most human color perceptions can be generated by a mixture of three colors called ''primaries''. This is used to reproduce color scenes in photography, printing, television, and other media. There are a number of methods or color spaces for specifying a color in terms of three particular primary colors. Each method has its advantages and disadvantages depending on the particular application. No mixture of colors, however, can produce a response truly identical to that of a spectral color, although one can get close, especially for the longer wavelengths, where the CIE 1931 color space chromaticity diagram has a nearly straight edge. For example, mixing green light (530 nm) and blue light (460 nm) produces cyan light that is slightly desaturated, because response of the red color receptor would be greater to the green and blue light in the mixture than it would be to a pure cyan light at 485 nm that has the same intensity as the mixture of blue and green. Because of this, and because the ''primaries'' in color printing systems generally are not pure themselves, the colors reproduced are never perfectly saturated spectral colors, and so spectral colors cannot be matched exactly. However, natural scenes rarely contain fully saturated colors, thus such scenes can usually be approximated well by these systems. The range of colors that can be reproduced with a given color reproduction system is called the gamut. The International Commission on Illumination, CIE chromaticity diagram can be used to describe the gamut. Another problem with color reproduction systems is connected with the initial measurement of color, or colorimetry. The characteristics of the color sensors in measurement devices (e.g. cameras, scanners) are often very far from the characteristics of the receptors in the human eye. A color reproduction system "tuned" to a human with normal color vision may give very inaccurate results for other observers, according to color vision deviations to the CIE 1931 color space#CIE standard observer, standard observer. The different color response of different devices can be problematic if not properly managed. For color information stored and transferred in digital form, color management techniques, such as those based on ICC profiles, can help to avoid distortions of the reproduced colors. Color management does not circumvent the gamut limitations of particular output devices, but can assist in finding good mapping of input colors into the gamut that can be reproduced.


Additive coloring

Additive color is light created by mixing together light of two or more different colors. Red, green, and blue are the additive primary colors normally used in additive color systems such as projectors and computer terminals.


Subtractive coloring

Subtractive coloring uses dyes, inks, pigments, or filters to absorb some wavelengths of light and not others. The color that a surface displays comes from the parts of the visible spectrum that are not absorbed and therefore remain visible. Without pigments or dye, fabric fibers, paint base and paper are usually made of particles that scatter white light (all colors) well in all directions. When a pigment or ink is added, wavelengths are absorbed or "subtracted" from white light, so light of another color reaches the eye. If the light is not a pure white source (the case of nearly all forms of artificial lighting), the resulting spectrum will appear a slightly different color. Red paint, viewed under blue light, may appear black. Red paint is red because it scatters only the red components of the spectrum. If red paint is illuminated by blue light, it will be absorbed by the red paint, creating the appearance of a black object.


Structural color

Structural colors are colors caused by interference effects rather than by pigments. Color effects are produced when a material is scored with fine parallel lines, formed of one or more parallel thin layers, or otherwise composed of microstructures on the scale of the color's wavelength. If the microstructures are spaced randomly, light of shorter wavelengths will be scattered preferentially to produce Tyndall effect colors: the blue of the sky (Rayleigh scattering, caused by structures much smaller than the wavelength of light, in this case, air molecules), the luster of opals, and the blue of human irises. If the microstructures are aligned in arrays, for example, the array of pits in a CD, they behave as a diffraction grating: the grating reflects different wavelengths in different directions due to wave interference, interference phenomena, separating mixed "white" light into light of different wavelengths. If the structure is one or more thin layers then it will reflect some wavelengths and transmit others, depending on the layers' thickness. Structural color is studied in the field of thin-film optics. The most ordered or the most changeable structural colors are iridescence, iridescent. Structural color is responsible for the blues and greens of the feathers of many birds (the blue jay, for example), as well as certain butterfly wings and beetle shells. Variations in the pattern's spacing often give rise to an iridescent effect, as seen in peafowl, peacock feathers, soap bubbles, films of oil, and nacre, mother of pearl, because the reflected color depends upon the viewing angle. Numerous scientists have carried out research in butterfly wings and beetle shells, including Isaac Newton and Robert Hooke. Since 1942, electron microscope, electron micrography has been used, advancing the development of products that exploit structural color, such as "photonic" cosmetics.


Additional terms

*Color wheel: an illustrative organization of color hues in a circle that shows relationships. *Colorfulness, chroma, purity, or saturation: how "intense" or "concentrated" a color is. Technical definitions distinguish between colorfulness, chroma, and saturation as distinct perceptual attributes and include purity as a physical quantity. These terms, and others related to light and color, are internationally agreed upon and published in the CIE Lighting Vocabulary. More readily available texts on colorimetry also define and explain these terms.R.S. Berns
Principles of Color Technology
, 3rd Ed., Wiley, New York (2001).
*Dichromatism: a phenomenon where the hue is dependent on the concentration and thickness of the absorbing substance. *Hue: the color's direction from white, for example in a color wheel or chromaticity diagram. *Tints and shades, Shade: a color made darker by adding black. *Tints and shades, Tint: a color made lighter by adding white. *Lightness, Value, brightness, lightness, or luminosity: how light or dark a color is.


See also

*Chromophore *Color analysis (art) *Color in Chinese culture *Color mapping *Complementary color *Impossible color *International Color Consortium *International Commission on Illumination *Lists of colors list of colors (compact), (compact version) *Neutral color *Pearlescent coating including Metal effect pigments *Pseudocolor *Primary color, Primary, secondary color, secondary and tertiary colors


References


External links


ColorLab
MATLAB toolbox for color science computation and accurate color reproduction (by Jesus Malo and Maria Jose Luque, Universitat de Valencia). It includes CIE standard tristimulus colorimetry and transformations to a number of non-linear color appearance models (CIE Lab, CIE CAM, etc.).

Buenos Aires University * * *Robert Ridgway'
''A Nomenclature of Colors'' (1886)
an
''Color Standards and Color Nomenclature'' (1912)
€”text-searchable digital facsimiles at Linda Hall Library *Albert Henry Munsell'
''A Color Notation''
(1907) at Project Gutenberg
AIC
International Colour Association
The Effect of Color , OFF BOOK
Documentary produced by Off Book (web series), Off Book
Study of the history of colors
{{Authority control Color, Image processing Qualia Vision