Causative verb
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In
linguistics Linguistics is the science, scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure ...
, a causative (
abbreviated An abbreviation (from Latin ''brevis'', meaning ''short'') is a shortened form of a word or phrase, by any method. It may consist of a group of letters or words taken from the full version of the word or phrase; for example, the word ''abbrevia ...
) is a valency-increasing operationPayne, Thomas E. (1997).
Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists
'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 173–186.
that indicates that a subject either causes someone or something else to do or be something or causes a change in state of a non- volitional event. Normally, it brings in a new argument (the causer), A, into a transitive clause, with the original subject S becoming the object O. All languages have ways to express causation but differ in the means. Most, if not all, languages have specific or ''lexical'' causative forms (such as English ''rise'' → ''raise'', ''lie'' → ''lay'', ''sit'' → ''set''). Some languages also have morphological devices (such as
inflection In linguistic morphology, inflection (or inflexion) is a process of word formation in which a word is modified to express different grammatical categories such as tense, case, voice, aspect, person, number, gender, mood, animacy, and ...
) that change verbs into their causative forms or change adjectives into verbs of ''becoming''. Other languages employ periphrasis, with control verbs, idiomatic expressions or
auxiliary verb An auxiliary verb ( abbreviated ) is a verb that adds functional or grammatical meaning to the clause in which it occurs, so as to express tense, aspect, modality, voice, emphasis, etc. Auxiliary verbs usually accompany an infinitive verb or a ...
s. There tends to be a link between how "compact" a causative device is and its semantic meaning. The normal English causative verb or control verb used in periphrasis is ''make'' rather than ''cause''. Linguistic terms are traditionally given names with a
Romance Romance (from Vulgar Latin , "in the Roman language", i.e., "Latin") may refer to: Common meanings * Romance (love), emotional attraction towards another person and the courtship behaviors undertaken to express the feelings * Romance languages, ...
root, which has led some to believe that ''cause'' is more prototypical. While ''cause'' is a causative, it carries some additional meaning (it implies direct causation) and is less common than ''make''. Also, while most other English causative verbs require a ''to'' complement clause (as in “My mom caused me to eat broccoli"), ''make'' does not require one ("My mom made me eat broccoli"), at least when it is not being used in the
passive voice A passive voice construction is a grammatical voice construction that is found in many languages. In a clause with passive voice, the grammatical subject expresses the ''theme'' or ''patient'' of the main verb – that is, the person or thing t ...
.


Terminology

Many authors have written extensively on causative constructions and have used a variety of terms, often to talk about the same things. S, A, and O are terms used in
morphosyntactic alignment In linguistics, morphosyntactic alignment is the grammatical relationship between arguments—specifically, between the two arguments (in English, subject and object) of transitive verbs like ''the dog chased the cat'', and the single argument ...
to describe
arguments An argument is a statement or group of statements called premises intended to determine the degree of truth or acceptability of another statement called conclusion. Arguments can be studied from three main perspectives: the logical, the dialectic ...
in a sentence. The subject of an
intransitive verb In grammar, an intransitive verb is a verb whose context does not entail a direct object. That lack of transitivity distinguishes intransitive verbs from transitive verbs, which entail one or more objects. Additionally, intransitive verbs are ...
is S, the agent of a transitive verb is A, and the object of a transitive is O. These terms are technically not abbreviations (anymore) for " subject", " agent", and " object", though they can usually be thought of that way. P is often used instead of O in many works. The term underlying is used to describe sentences, phrases, or words that correspond to their causative versions. Often, this underlying sentence may not be explicitly stated. For example, for the sentence "'John made Bill drive the truck'", the underlying sentence would be ''Bill drove the truck''. This has also been called the base situation.Lehmann, Christian (2013). “Latin causativization in typological perspective”. In Lenoble, Muriel & Longrée, Dominique (eds.) (forthcoming), ''Actes du 13ème Colloque International de Linguistique Latine''. Louvain: Peeters. A derived sentence would be the causativized variant of the underlying sentence. The causer is the new argument in a causative expression that causes the action to be done. The causer is the new argument brought into a derived sentence. In the example sentence above, ''John'' is the causer. The causee is the argument that actually does the action in a causativized sentence. It is usually present in both the underlying and derived sentences. ''Bill'' is the causee in the above example.


Devices

There are various ways of encoding causation, which form somewhat of a continuum of "compactness."Dixon, R.M.W. 2000. “A typology of causatives: form, syntax and meaning”. In ''Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity'', Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds. New York: Cambridge University Press. p.30–83.


Lexical

Lexical causatives are common in the world's languages. There are three kinds of lexical causatives, the unifying factor being that the idea of causation is part of the semantics of the verb itself. (English, for example, employs all three of these kinds of lexical causatives.) On the surface, lexical causatives look essentially the same as a regular transitive verb. There are a few reasons why this is not true. The first is that transitive verbs generally do not have an intransitive counterpart but lexical causatives do. The semantics of the verbs show the difference as well. A regular transitive verb implies a single event while a lexical causative implies a realization of an event: :(a) John kicked the ice but nothing happened to it. :(b) *John melted the ice but nothing happened to it. Sentence (b) is judged ungrammatical because it goes against the successful event implied by the verb ''melt''.


One word

Some languages, including English, have
ambitransitive verb An ambitransitive verb is a verb that is both intransitive and transitive.Dixon, R.M.W. & Aikhenvald, Alexendra Y. Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity. Cambridge University Press. This verb may or may not require a direct object. Englis ...
s like ''break'', ''burn'' or ''awake'', which may either be
intransitive In grammar, an intransitive verb is a verb whose context does not entail a direct object. That lack of transitivity distinguishes intransitive verbs from transitive verbs, which entail one or more objects. Additionally, intransitive verbs ar ...
or transitive ("The vase broke" vs. "I broke the vase.") These are split into two varieties: agentive and patientive ambitransitives. Agentive ambitransitives (also called S=A ambitransitives) include verbs such as ''walk'' and ''knit'' because the S of the intransitive corresponds to the A of the transitive. For example: :(1a) ''Mary'' (S) ''is knitting.'' :(1b) ''Mary'' (A) ''is knitting a scarf'' (O). This type of ambitransitive does ''not'' show a causative relationship. For patientive ambitransitives (also called S=O ambitransitives), such as ''trip'' and ''spill'', the S of the intransitive corresponds to the O of the transitive: :(2a) ''The milk '' (S) ''spilled.'' :(2b) ''Jim'' (A) ''spilled the milk'' (O). These are further divided into two more types, based on speakers' intuition. Some, like ''spill'' in (2), are primarily transitive and secondarily intransitive. Other verbs like this include ''smash'' and ''extend.'' Other verbs, such as ''trip'' in (3) go the other way: they are primarily intransitive and secondarily transitive. :(3a) ''John'' (S) ''tripped.'' :(3b) ''Mary'' (A) ''tripped John'' (O). Other examples of this type include ''explode'', ''melt'', ''dissolve'', ''walk'', and ''march''. It is this type of ambitransitive verb that is considered a causative This is given some anecdotal evidence in that to translate (3b) above into languages with morphological causatives, a morpheme would need to be attached to the verb. Lexical causatives are apparently constrained to involving only one agentive argument. Semantically, the causer is usually marked as the patient. In fact, it is unlikely whether any language has a lexical causative for verbs such as "swim," "sing," "read," or "kick."


Irregular stem change

English ''fell'' (as in "Paul felled the tree") can be thought of as a lexical causative of ''fall'' ("the tree fell"), exemplifying this category. This is considered a lexical change because it is not at all productive. If it were productive, it would be an internal change morphological causative (below).


Two words

English has verb pairs such as ''rise'' and ''raise'', ''eat'' and ''feed'', ''see'' and ''show'' where one is essentially the causative correspondent of the other. These pairs are linked semantically by various means, usually involving translation. For example, ''burn'', as in ''The grass burned'' (intransitive ), would translate as ''awa-'' in Yimas while ''burn'', as in ''I burned the grass'' (transitive), would translate as ''ampu-'' in Yimas


Morphological

There are eight different morphological processes by which a causative may be marked, roughly organized by compactness: Within morphological causatives, this degree of compactness bears an important variable when considering the semantics of the two processes. For example, mechanisms that do not change the length of the word (internal change, tone change) are shorter than those that lengthen it. Of those that lengthen it, shorter changes are more compact than longer. Verbs can be classified into four categories, according to how susceptible they are to morphological causativization: # Inactive intransitives (''faint'') # Middle/ingestive verbs (either intransitive or transitive such as ''sit down'', ''ascend'', ''put clothes on'', ''eat'', or ''learn'') # Active intransitives (''work'') # Transitive verbs (''carry'') This hierarchy has some exceptions, but it does generally hold true. For example, given a text of Guarani, only about 16% of causatives apply to transitives. For some languages, it may not apply to transitive verbs productively and may only apply to verbs that denote abstract action or consumption of food. Additionally, within Athabaskan family, all languages can causativize inactive intransitives, but not all of them can causativize active intransitives or even transitives.


Two verbs in one predicate

A number of languages involve a form of analytic causative that involves two verbs in a single predicate, such as French,
Spanish Spanish might refer to: * Items from or related to Spain: **Spaniards are a nation and ethnic group indigenous to Spain **Spanish language, spoken in Spain and many Latin American countries **Spanish cuisine Other places * Spanish, Ontario, Can ...
,
Italian Italian(s) may refer to: * Anything of, from, or related to the people of Italy over the centuries ** Italians, an ethnic group or simply a citizen of the Italian Republic or Italian Kingdom ** Italian language, a Romance language *** Regional Ita ...
and Catalan. For example, when French ''faire'' is used as a causative, the causee noun phrase cannot occur between it and the next verb. Unlike most other
Romance languages The Romance languages, sometimes referred to as Latin languages or Neo-Latin languages, are the various modern languages that evolved from Vulgar Latin. They are the only extant subgroup of the Italic languages in the Indo-European language ...
,
Portuguese Portuguese may refer to: * anything of, from, or related to the country and nation of Portugal ** Portuguese cuisine, traditional foods ** Portuguese language, a Romance language *** Portuguese dialects, variants of the Portuguese language ** Portu ...
uses a periphrastic construction like that of English, discussed below.
Kiowa Kiowa () people are a Native American tribe and an indigenous people of the Great Plains of the United States. They migrated southward from western Montana into the Rocky Mountains in Colorado in the 17th and 18th centuries,Pritzker 326 and e ...
uses a similar mechanism. Verbs can be compounded with the transitive verb ''ɔ́m'' to create a causative:


Periphrastic constructions

Some languages use a periphrastic (or analytic) construction to express causation and typically include two verbs and two clauses. English causatives prototypically use ''make'' (but other verbs such as ''cause'', ''order'', ''allow'', ''force'', ''compel'' can be used) in the main clause with the lexical verb in a subordinate clause, as in "I made him go." Other languages, such as
Persian Persian may refer to: * People and things from Iran, historically called ''Persia'' in the English language ** Persians, the majority ethnic group in Iran, not to be conflated with the Iranic peoples ** Persian language, an Iranian language of the ...
, have the opposite syntax: the causative is in a subordinating clause and the main verb is in the main clause, as in the following example from Macushi: Canela-Krahô has a combination of the two in which the causee is marked twice, once in each clause: Portuguese also has a periphrastic construction like that of English but unlike most other Romance languages: Analytic causatives are sometimes not considered to be valency increasing devices, but they can semantically be interpreted as such.


Semantics

A language may have one or more different formal mechanisms for expression causation. For languages with only one, the semantic range is broad. For those with multiple, there is always a semantic difference between the two. R.M.W. Dixon breaks down these semantic differences into 9 parameters, involving the verb itself, the causee, and the causer: :(a) Parameters that relate to the verb itself ::*1. State/Action: Can the causative apply to state and process verbs or does it apply to action verbs? ::*2. Transitivity: Does the causative apply to only intransitives, to intransitives and some transitives, or to all verbs? :(b) Parameters that relate to the thing being caused (the original S or A) ::*3. Control: Does the causee have control of the activity? ::*4. Volition: Does the causee do the action willingly or unwillingly? ::*5. Affectedness: Is the causee completely or partially affected? :(c) Parameters that relate to the causer (the new A in a causative construction) ::*6. Directness: Does the causer act directly or indirectly? ::*7. Intention: Is the result achieved accidentally or intentionally? ::*8. Naturalness: Does the activity happen fairly naturally or is it with effort, violence, or force? ::*9. Involvement: How involved was the causer in the activity? These parameters are not mutually exclusive. Many causative constructions involve the semantics of two or more parameters. However, the difference between the causatives in a language most likely will be distinguished by one of the parameters.


Relationship between devices and semantics


Animacy of the object

There is a strong correlation between the
semantics Semantics (from grc, σημαντικός ''sēmantikós'', "significant") is the study of reference, meaning, or truth. The term can be used to refer to subfields of several distinct disciplines, including philosophy, linguistics and comp ...
of a causative and the mechanism by which it is expressed. Generally, if a causative is more "compact" than another, it usually implies a more direct causation. So, for inanimate and unconscious objects, English analytic causatives (1–3) are not completely synonymous with lexical causatives (4–6): # "I made the tree fall." # "I made the chicken die." # "I made the cup rise to my lips." # "I felled the tree." # "I killed the chicken." # "I raised the cup to my lips." Analytic causatives (1–3) imply that no physical contact was involved and therefore was done by some sort of magical power or
telekinesis Psychokinesis (from grc, ψυχή, , soul and grc, κίνησις, , movement, label=ㅤ), or telekinesis (from grc, τηλε, , far off and grc, κίνησις, , movement, label=ㅤ), is a hypothetical psychic ability allowing a person ...
. Lexical causatives (4–6) do not imply this meaning.Haiman, John (1983). "Iconic and Economic Motivation". ''Language''. 59:4 pp. 781–819. For animate and conscious objects, there is a different difference in meaning: # "He caused them to lie down." # "He laid them down." (1) only makes sense if ''they'' are animate and awake. Barring magic, (2) only makes sense if the object is inanimate or unconscious.


Finite and non-finite verbs

Generally, the more distance between the causer and the causee, the more finite the verb. Consider the following examples from
Spanish Spanish might refer to: * Items from or related to Spain: **Spaniards are a nation and ethnic group indigenous to Spain **Spanish language, spoken in Spain and many Latin American countries **Spanish cuisine Other places * Spanish, Ontario, Can ...
: The first example implies that Montezuma was physically there and was directly involved in making Cortés eat bread. The second example implies that Montezuma was not physically there and arranged for something to happen to make Cortés eat bread, perhaps by killing all of his cattle. That could approximate the English construction "Montezuma got Cortés to eat bread." Therefore, at least in Spanish, a conjugated verb implies a more direct causation.


Dixon's prototypes

Dixon examines this correlation cross-linguistically, and summarizes his findings in the following table. In this table, L refers to lexical causatives, M1 refers to more compact morphological processes while M2 refers to less compact processes, CP refers to complex
predicates Predicate or predication may refer to: * Predicate (grammar), in linguistics * Predication (philosophy) * several closely related uses in mathematics and formal logic: **Predicate (mathematical logic) **Propositional function **Finitary relation, ...
(two verbs, one predicate), and P refers to periphrastic constructions. These processes are explained more clearly in the devices section above. Parameter 9, Involvement, cannot be included in the table because the only two languages with this distinction, Nomatsiguenga and Kamayurá, the morphemes are about the same length. When a larger sample of languages show this distinction, perhaps this parameter can be included in the table. The table shows that for each of eight semantic parameters outlined in the
semantics Semantics (from grc, σημαντικός ''sēmantikós'', "significant") is the study of reference, meaning, or truth. The term can be used to refer to subfields of several distinct disciplines, including philosophy, linguistics and comp ...
section above, more compact causative processes show one distinction while less compact processes show the other distinction. For example, Parameter 6 distinguishes between more direct and less direct causation. In
Hindi Hindi ( Devanāgarī: or , ), or more precisely Modern Standard Hindi (Devanagari: ), is an Indo-Aryan language spoken chiefly in the Hindi Belt region encompassing parts of northern, central, eastern, and western India. Hindi has been ...
, M1, or the shorter morphological process, shows direct causation while M2, the longer morphological process, shows indirect causation. Summarizing the table, Dixon has given two prototypes for causatives: :Prototype 1 ::*Causer achieves the result natural, intentionally, and directly ::*Causee either lacking control or being willing and may be partially affected ::*Less transitive verbs affected :Prototype 2 ::*Causer achieves the result accidentally, with effort, or acts indirectly ::*Causee is in control but unwilling and is completely affected. ::*More likely to apply to all types of verbs All eight of the components in each prototype are never attested in a single causative. However, a single process may have two or three components. Dixon admits to these being very tentative and in need for further investigation.


Syntax

R.M.W. Dixon also outlines the syntactic possibilities of causatives in the world's languages.


Intransitives

Since
intransitive verb In grammar, an intransitive verb is a verb whose context does not entail a direct object. That lack of transitivity distinguishes intransitive verbs from transitive verbs, which entail one or more objects. Additionally, intransitive verbs are ...
s have low valency, virtually any type of causative construction can apply to them productively within a language. Some constructions are ''only'' allowed with intransitive verbs and some languages (such as
Arabic Arabic (, ' ; , ' or ) is a Semitic language spoken primarily across the Arab world.Semitic languages: an international handbook / edited by Stefan Weninger; in collaboration with Geoffrey Khan, Michael P. Streck, Janet C. E.Watson; Walter ...
, Blackfoot, and Gothic) only allow causatives of intransitive verbs, with some exceptions. In all cases, the original subject of the underlying intransitive verb corresponds with the object of the derived transitive verb. All languages have this construction, though some allow a semantic difference if the original subject is marked differently (such as
Japanese Japanese may refer to: * Something from or related to Japan, an island country in East Asia * Japanese language, spoken mainly in Japan * Japanese people, the ethnic group that identifies with Japan through ancestry or culture ** Japanese diaspor ...
and Hungarian). For split systems, causatives of intransitives may be treated differently. The syntax of a causative construction is almost always the same as some other type of sentence, such as a sentence with a transitive verb. Tariana, however, is an exception to this rule.


Transitives

In the causative of a
transitive verb A transitive verb is a verb that accepts one or more objects, for example, 'cleaned' in ''Donald cleaned the window''. This contrasts with intransitive verbs, which do not have objects, for example, 'panicked' in ''Donald panicked''. Transiti ...
, the new causer always becomes the new A of the sentence. What happens to the causee and the original object depend on the language. Dixon shows that there are five main types of situations: : Within type (v) there are two main subtypes. Either the original A goes into the first empty slot in a hierarchy or it always takes a certain function. For the first subtype, there is a hierarchy involved in the language: :subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique > genitive > object of comparison.Comrie, B. (1975). "Causatives and universal grammar," ''Transactions of the Philological Society for 1974.'' p. 1–32. French is a language that follows this hierarchy. When a causative is employed, the original A does not get marked the same for intransitives, transitives, and ditransitives. In this first example, the verb in intransitive, and with the subject slot taken, the original A becomes a direct object: The following example has a transitive verb. The subject and direct object slots are filled (with ''je'' and ''les gâteaux'', respectively) so the original A becomes an indirect object: This final French example has a ditransitive verb. The subject is ''je'', the direct object is ''une lettre'', and the indirect object is ''directeur'', so the original A is marked as an oblique: While some writers have called this hierarchical causative construction the norm, outside of
Romance language The Romance languages, sometimes referred to as Latin languages or Neo-Latin languages, are the various modern languages that evolved from Vulgar Latin. They are the only extant subgroup of the Italic languages in the Indo-European language f ...
s it is in fact rather rare. Most other languages are of the second subtype of type (v), and the original A takes on a set case or marking, regardless whether the underlying verb is intransitive or transitive: :


Ditransitives

The syntactic and morphological constraints of individual language generally restrict causatives of ditransitive verbs. The underlying phrase already contains an A, O, and
indirect object In linguistics, an object is any of several types of arguments. In subject-prominent, nominative-accusative languages such as English, a transitive verb typically distinguishes between its subject and any of its objects, which can include but ...
, and so in order to accommodate a fourth argument, languages employ a variety of constructions. They tend to be idiosyncratic and are difficult to group together into types. Additionally, data is patchy for many languages since descriptions of languages seldom include information of causatives of ditransitives.


Double causatives

Some types of causative constructions essentially do not permit double causatives, ''e.g.'' it would be difficult to find a lexical double causative. Periphrastic causatives however, have the potential to always be applied iteratively (''Mom made Dad make my brother make his friends leave the house.''). Many Indo-Aryan languages (such as Hindustani) have lexical double causatives. For morphological causatives, some languages do not allow single morpheme to be applied twice on a single verb ( Jarawara) while others do ( Capanawa, Hungarian, Turkish, Kabardian, Karbi), though sometimes with an idiomatic meaning ( Swahili's means ''force to do'' and Oromo's carries an intensive meaning). Other languages, such as Nivkh, have two different morphological mechanisms that can apply to a single verb. Still others have one morpheme that applies to intransitives and another to transitives ( Apalai, Guarani). All of these examples apply to underlying intransitive verbs, yielding a ditransitive verb. So far, there are no reliable data for a morphological double causative of a transitive verb, resulting in a verb with four arguments.


Other topics


Causative (repetitive)

Yokuts The Yokuts (previously known as MariposasPowell, 1891:90–91.) are an ethnic group of Native Americans native to central California. Before European contact, the Yokuts consisted of up to 60 tribes speaking several related languages. ''Yokuts ...
, an indigenous language spoken in California, has a morpheme, ''-lsaˑ'', that indicates causation in addition to repetition. This is separate from the language's normal mechanisms of causation. This implies a single act by the causer, but multiple acts by the causee.


Causative voice

The causative voice is a
grammatical voice In linguistics, grammaticality is determined by the conformity to language usage as derived by the grammar of a particular speech variety. The notion of grammaticality rose alongside the theory of generative grammar, the goal of which is to form ...
promoting the oblique argument of a transitive verb to an actor argument. When the causative voice is applied to a verb, its valency increases by one. If, after the application of the grammatical voice, there are two actor arguments, one of them is obligatorily demoted to an oblique argument.
Japanese Japanese may refer to: * Something from or related to Japan, an island country in East Asia * Japanese language, spoken mainly in Japan * Japanese people, the ethnic group that identifies with Japan through ancestry or culture ** Japanese diaspor ...
, Turkish and Mongolian are examples of languages with the causative voice. The following are examples from Japanese:


Causal case

The causal or causative case (
abbreviated An abbreviation (from Latin ''brevis'', meaning ''short'') is a shortened form of a word or phrase, by any method. It may consist of a group of letters or words taken from the full version of the word or phrase; for example, the word ''abbrevia ...
) is a
grammatical case A grammatical case is a category of nouns and noun modifiers ( determiners, adjectives, participles, and numerals), which corresponds to one or more potential grammatical functions for a nominal group in a wording. In various languages, nomin ...
that indicates that the marked noun is the cause or reason for something. It is found in the
Dravidian languages The Dravidian languages (or sometimes Dravidic) are a family of languages spoken by 250 million people, mainly in southern India, north-east Sri Lanka, and south-west Pakistan. Since the colonial era, there have been small but significant ...
Kannada Kannada (; ಕನ್ನಡ, ), originally romanised Canarese, is a Dravidian language spoken predominantly by the people of Karnataka in southwestern India, with minorities in all neighbouring states. It has around 47 million native s ...
and Telugu, the Native South American language
Quechua Quechua may refer to: *Quechua people, several indigenous ethnic groups in South America, especially in Peru *Quechuan languages, a Native South American language family spoken primarily in the Andes, derived from a common ancestral language **So ...
, and Northeast Caucasian Archi. It is also found in extinct Tocharian B, an Indo-European language.


Causal-final case

The causal-final is a
grammatical case A grammatical case is a category of nouns and noun modifiers ( determiners, adjectives, participles, and numerals), which corresponds to one or more potential grammatical functions for a nominal group in a wording. In various languages, nomin ...
in Hungarian (and Chuvash) expressing the meaning 'for the purpose of, for the reason that',Rounds, C. (2001). ''Hungarian: an essential grammar''. Routledge. and denoting price asked of or paid for goods. It is formed by adding the ending suffix ''-ért'' to the end of the noun, e.g. ''kenyér'' "bread" >''kenyérért'' "for bread", e.g. ''elküldtem a boltba kenyérért'' "I sent him to the store for bread". It is not affected by
vowel harmony In phonology, vowel harmony is an assimilatory process in which the vowels of a given domain – typically a phonological word – have to be members of the same natural class (thus "in harmony"). Vowel harmony is typically long distance, mea ...
in Hungarian.


Literature


Shibatani

ShibataniShibatani, M., ed. (2001) ''The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation''. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. lists three criteria for entities and relations that must be encoded in linguistic expressions of causation: # An agent causing or forcing another participant to perform an action, or to be in a certain condition # The relation between hetwo events the causing event, and the caused performing/being eventis such that the speaker believes that the occurrence of one event, the ‟caused event,” has been realized at t2, which is after t1, the time of the ‟causing event” # The relation between causing event and caused event is such that the speaker believes the occurrence of the caused event depends wholly on the occurrence of the causing event—the dependency of the two events here must be to the extent that it allows the speaker a counterfactual inference that the caused event would not have taken place at a particular time if the causing event had not taken place, provided that all else had remained the same. This set of definitional prerequisites allows for a broad set of types of relationships based, at least, on the lexical verb, the semantics of the causer, the semantics of the causee and the semantics of the construction explicitly encoding the causal relationship. Many analysts (Comrie (1981), Song (1996), Dixon (2000) and others) have worked to tease apart what factors (semantic or otherwise) account for the distribution of causative constructions, as well as to document what patterns actually occur cross-linguistically.


Comrie

Bernard Comrie Bernard Sterling Comrie, (; born 23 May 1947) is a British-born linguist. Comrie is a specialist in linguistic typology, linguistic universals and on Caucasian languages. Early life and education Comrie was born in Sunderland, England on 23 ...
Comrie, B. (1981). ''Language Universals and Linguistic Typology: Syntax and Morphology''. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p.158–177 focuses on the typology of the syntax and semantics of causative constructions proper. Crucially, Comrie (and others to be discussed here) distinguish between the linguistic encoding of causal relations and other extra-linguistic concerns such as the nature of causation itself and questions of how humans perceive of causal relations. While certainly not irrelevant, these extra-linguistic questions will, for now, be left aside. Comrie usefully characterizes causative events in terms of two (or more) microevents perceived of composing a macroevent, and encoded in a single expression (of varying size and form). Formally, he categorizes causatives into 3 types, depending on the contiguity of the material encoding the causing event and that encoding the caused event. These are: 1) lexical causatives, in which the two events are expressed in a single lexical item, as in the well-discussed case of English kill; 2) morphological causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in a single verbal complex via causative morphology, and, prototypically, morphological marking showing the status of affected arguments. Finally, Comrie discusses analytic causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in separate clauses. Comrie's work is also noteworthy for having brought the notion of syntactic hierarchy to bear on the typology of causative constructions. A hierarchy of grammatical relations had already been formulated to help explain possibilities for relative clause formation (first presented as Keenan and Comrie's (1972) NP accessibility hierarchy; see Croft 1990: 147), and Comrie argued that a similar hierarchy was in play, at least in some constructions, in the marking of the original A argument when a base transitive clause is causativized. The hierarchy is as follows: *subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique > genitive Comrie's argument was, in short, that some causativized-transitive constructions mark the new A as belonging to the leftmost available slot in the above hierarchy. Dixon (2000) fleshes out a version this analysis in more detail.


Song

Presenting a typology of causatives and causation based on a database of 600 languages, SongSong, J.J. (1996). ''Causatives and causation: A universal-typological perspective''. London and New York: Addison Wesley Longman. is very critical of typological work that depends on statistical inference, citing data from the Niger-Congo family that contradicts some earlier claims that “languages within genera are generally fairly similar typologically”. Song therefore culls data from every language for which adequate documentation is available to him, and categorizes the various causative constructions gleaned therefrom into three classes: COMPACT, AND and PURP. Song employs the following terminology: * cause– the clause which denotes a causing event * effect– the clause which denotes the caused event * cause– verbal elements of cause* effect verbal elements of effectref name=Song /> The major differences between Song's analysis and Comrie (1981) and Dixon (2000), is that Song lumps the range of lexical and morphological causatives together under the label COMPACT, in which causecan be “less than a free morpheme” (e.g., bound morpheme refix, suffix, infix, circumfix, reduplication zero-derivation, suppletion); or “a free morpheme”, in which causeand effectform a single grammatical unit. Most of the examples given look like serial verb constructions, and no in-depth analysis is undertaken for some of the constructions in which causeand effectare less formally contiguous. Song notes this non-contiguity, but does not undertake to explain why it might be important. The AND causative, for Song, is any construction with a separate causeand effect i.e., in which “two clauses reinvolved”. This, in theory, could include larger, multi-clausal expressions of causal relations which many analysts probably would not label a ‘causative construction’, e.g.: ‘It rained yesterday, so they stayed home’, but the boundaries of the AND causative category are not discussed. One of Song's major contributions to the literature is fleshing out an analysis of his PURP causative. These are constructions which encode intended causation on the part of the causer, but which do not encode any outcome: i.e., the speaker encodes causeand causer intentionality, but remains agnostic as to whether effectwas felicitously effected.


Talmy

Leonard Talmy Leonard Talmy is an emeritus professor of linguistics Linguistics is the scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of ...
Talmy, L. 2000. ''Toward a Cognitive Semantics'' Volume 2: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge: MIT Press. p.67–101 conducts an in-depth investigation of different types of causal relations. Talmy refers to these as “lexicalization patterns,” a term which remains unclear to me, given that few of the examples given in his discussion are lexical items, and most interpretations of “different types of causation incorporated in the verb root” are in fact wholly dependent on other morphosyntactic material in the clause. Let us first examine his list of possible (semantic) causative types, with examples: *autonomous events (non-causative) ''The vase broke.'' *resulting-event causation ''The vase broke from a ball’s rolling into it.'' *causing-event causation ''A ball’s rolling into it broke the vase.'' *instrument causation ''A ball broke the vase.'' *author causation (unintended) ''I broke the vase in rolling a ball into it.'' *agent causation (intended) ''I broke the vase by rolling a ball into it.'' *undergoer situation (non-causative) ''My arm broke (on me) when I fell.'' *self-agentive causation ''I walked to the store.'' *caused agency (inductive causation) ''I sent him to the store.'' One question remaining to be explored is how this set of divisions usefully differs from other analysts’ typologies of the semantics of encoding causal relations. Some overlap in the types of semantic information in play is immediately apparent, however: in cases of instrument causation (‘the hammer broke the cup’), we would certainly expect the ‘causer’ to be acting directly ixon's criterion 6and to be involved in the activity riterion 9 likewise, we would expect instances of caused agency to include more information on causee control on willingness riteria 3 & 4


Indo-European languages


Germanic languages


Proto-Germanic

In
Proto-Germanic Proto-Germanic (abbreviated PGmc; also called Common Germanic) is the reconstructed proto-language of the Germanic branch of the Indo-European languages. Proto-Germanic eventually developed from pre-Proto-Germanic into three Germanic bran ...
, the parent language of
Germanic languages The Germanic languages are a branch of the Indo-European language family spoken natively by a population of about 515 million people mainly in Europe, North America, Oceania and Southern Africa. The most widely spoken Germanic language, E ...
such as English, causative verbs are formed by adding a suffix ''-j/ij-'' to the past-tense ablaut of a strong verb, with
Verner's Law Verner's law describes a historical sound change in the Proto-Germanic language whereby consonants that would usually have been the voiceless fricatives , , , , , following an unstressed syllable, became the voiced fricatives , , , , . The law w ...
voicing applied. (All of those characteristics derive from the way that causative verbs are formed in
Proto-Indo-European Proto-Indo-European (PIE) is the reconstructed common ancestor of the Indo-European language family. Its proposed features have been derived by linguistic reconstruction from documented Indo-European languages. No direct record of Proto-Indo- ...
, with an accented ''-éy-'' suffix added to the ''o''-grade of a non-derived verb.) Here are some examples: *''*rīsaną'' (I) "to rise" → ''*raizijaną'' "to raise", i.e. "to cause to rise" *''*frawerþaną'' (III) "to perish" → ''*frawardijaną'' "to destroy", i.e. "to cause to perish" *''*nesaną'' (V) "to survive" → ''*nazjaną'' "to save", i.e. "to cause to survive" *''*ligjaną'' (V) "to lie down" → ''*lagjaną'' "to lay": "to cause to lie down" *''*grētaną'' (VII) "to weep" → ''*grōtijaną'' "to cause to weep" In English, ''to sit''/''to seat"'', and in German, ''sitzen''/''setzen'' form pairs of causatives/resultatives.


English

English uses various causative mechanisms, with varying degrees of
productivity Productivity is the efficiency of production of goods or services expressed by some measure. Measurements of productivity are often expressed as a ratio of an aggregate output to a single input or an aggregate input used in a production proces ...
. There are a large number of lexical causatives, such as ''kill'', ''open'' and ''feed''. Additionally, there are several morphemes that can express causation. For example, ''-(i)fy'' can be thought of as a causative in that it is a
derivation Derivation may refer to: Language * Morphological derivation, a word-formation process * Parse tree or concrete syntax tree, representing a string's syntax in formal grammars Law * Derivative work, in copyright law * Derivation proceeding, a proc ...
that turns an adjective or noun into a "verb of becoming": *''simple'' → ''simplify'' = "to make simple", "to cause (something) to become simple" *''object'' → ''objectify'' = "to make into an object", "to cause (something) to become an object" (figuratively, that is) ''en-'' can also be a causative. In English, adjectives (or ''stative verbs'' in other languages) can express the acquisition of a quality or changes of state with causatives, in the same way as with regular verbs. For example, if there is a stative verb ''to be large'', the causative will mean ''to enlarge'', ''to make grow''. The reflexive form of the causative can then be used to mean ''to enlarge oneself'', or even as a middle voice, ''to grow''. As far as lexical causatives are concerned, English has at least 49 causative verbs. Roughly half affect only sentient beings: ''allow'', ''block'', ''cause'', ''enable'', ''force'', ''get'', ''help'', ''hinder'', ''hold'', ''impede'', ''keep'', ''leave'', ''let'', ''make'', ''permit'', ''prevent'', ''protect'', ''restrain'', ''save'', ''set'', ''start'', ''stimulate'', ''stop''. The others can affect either sentient or non-sentient beings: ''aid'', ''bar'', ''bribe'', ''compel'', ''constrain'', ''convince'', ''deter'', ''discourage'', ''dissuade'', ''drive'', ''have'', ''hamper'', ''impel'', ''incite'', ''induce'', ''influence'', ''inspire'', ''lead'', ''move'', ''persuade'', ''prompt'', ''push'', ''restrict'', ''rouse'', ''send'', ''spur''.


Sanskrit

In
Sanskrit Sanskrit (; attributively , ; nominally , , ) is a classical language belonging to the Indo-Aryan branch of the Indo-European languages. It arose in South Asia after its predecessor languages had diffused there from the northwest in the late ...
, there is a causative form of the verb (''ṇijanta''), which is used when the subject of a clause forces or makes the object perform an action. The causative suffix ''-ay'' is attached to the verbal root, which may cause vowel
sandhi Sandhi ( sa, सन्धि ' , "joining") is a cover term for a wide variety of sound changes that occur at morpheme or word boundaries. Examples include fusion of sounds across word boundaries and the alteration of one sound depending on near ...
to take place: *''bhū'' "to be, exist" → ''bhāv-ay''; for example, ''bhāvayati'' "he causes to be" *''khad'' "to eat" → ''khād-ay''; for example, ''khādayāmi'' "I cause to eat" = "I feed"


Persian

In
Persian Persian may refer to: * People and things from Iran, historically called ''Persia'' in the English language ** Persians, the majority ethnic group in Iran, not to be conflated with the Iranic peoples ** Persian language, an Iranian language of the ...
, the causative form of the verb is formed by adding ''ân(i)dan'' to the present stem: *''xordan'' (to eat) → ''xor'' (present stem) → ''xorândan'' (to cause/make to eat) *''xandidan'' (to laugh) → ''xand'' (present stem) → ''xandândan'' (to cause/make to laugh)


Lithuanian

In Lithuanian, the causative form of the verb is made by adding suffix ''-in-'' to the present stem: *''skraidyti'' (to fly) → ''skraidinti'' (to make to fly) *''sėdėti'' (to sit) → ''sodinti'' (to make to sit) *''juoktis'' (to laugh) → ''juokinti'' (to make to laugh)


Latin

The topic of causatives has not been studied much for
Latin Latin (, or , ) is a classical language belonging to the Italic branch of the Indo-European languages. Latin was originally a dialect spoken in the lower Tiber area (then known as Latium) around present-day Rome, but through the power of the ...
, mainly because of its lack of productive morphological causative.


Hindustani

Hindustani uses the infix ''-(l)ā-'' and ''-(l)vā-'' to make verbs causative. *''karnā'' "to do" → ''karānā'' "to have done" → "karvānā" → "to have someone make someone do." *''paṛhnā'' "to read" → ''paṛhānā'' "to make someone read" → "paṛhvānā" "to cause someone to make someone read." *''hilnā'' "to move" → ''hilānā'' "to have something moved" → ''hilvānā'' "to have someone make something move." *''pīnā'' "to drink" → ''pilānā'' "to have someone drink" → ''pilvānā'' "to have someone make someone drink": “Usne naukrānī se bachchõ-ko pānī ''pilvāyā''” - "She had the maid make the kids drink water."


Bengali

The causative verbs are called () in Bengali. In the simplest way, the causative form of a verb can be formed by adding the suffix with the verbal noun form of the given verb. * 'to see' → 'to show/to cause someone to see'. * 'to eat' → 'to feed/to cause someone to eat'. From the verbal root ( in Bengali) perspective, the formation of causatives is done by adding the suffix with the verb roots ending with a consonant, and the suffix with those roots ending with a vowel. Thus, the verbal root transformations of the two previously mentioned verbs are: * → * → These verb roots are thereafter inflected with tense, aspect and mood.


Basque

The
Basque Basque may refer to: * Basques, an ethnic group of Spain and France * Basque language, their language Places * Basque Country (greater region), the homeland of the Basque people with parts in both Spain and France * Basque Country (autonomous co ...
language has two ways to form causative verbs: by using a non-ergative transitive verb in the absolute form, or by the morphological causativization. The first method is only possible with a restricted set of verbs which excludes those whose subjects take the ergative case, such as the verb ''eztul egin'' (cough -- literally "make (a) cough").


Turkish

In addition to very productive morphological causatives, Turkish also has some lexical causatives: ''kır-'' "break", ''yırt-'' "split", ''dik-'' "plant", ''yak-'' "burn", ''sakla-'' "hide", ''aç-'' "open".


Semitic languages

In most
Semitic languages The Semitic languages are a branch of the Afroasiatic language family. They are spoken by more than 330 million people across much of West Asia, the Horn of Africa, and latterly North Africa, Malta, West Africa, Chad, and in large immigra ...
, there is a causative form of the verb. It is postulated that in
Proto-Semitic Proto-Semitic is the hypothetical reconstructed proto-language ancestral to the Semitic languages. There is no consensus regarding the location of the Proto-Semitic '' Urheimat''; scholars hypothesize that it may have originated in the Levant ( ...
, the causative verbal stem was formed by the ''š-'' prefix, which has become ''ʾa-'', ''hi-'' or ''ī-'' in different languages. *
Syriac Syriac may refer to: *Syriac language, an ancient dialect of Middle Aramaic *Sureth, one of the modern dialects of Syriac spoken in the Nineveh Plains region * Syriac alphabet ** Syriac (Unicode block) ** Syriac Supplement * Neo-Aramaic languages a ...
: ''kəθav'' "he wrote" → ''ʾaxtev'' "he composed" *
Arabic Arabic (, ' ; , ' or ) is a Semitic language spoken primarily across the Arab world.Semitic languages: an international handbook / edited by Stefan Weninger; in collaboration with Geoffrey Khan, Michael P. Streck, Janet C. E.Watson; Walter ...
: ''ʿalima'' "he knew" → ''ʾaʿlama'' "he informed" *
Hebrew Hebrew (; ; ) is a Northwest Semitic language of the Afroasiatic language family. Historically, it is one of the spoken languages of the Israelites and their longest-surviving descendants, the Jews and Samaritans. It was largely preserved ...
: ''ṣaħak'' "he laughed" → ''hiṣħik'' "he made someone laugh" Arabic also has a causative form (Form II) created by
gemination In phonetics and phonology, gemination (), or consonant lengthening (from Latin 'doubling', itself from ''gemini'' 'twins'), is an articulation of a consonant for a longer period of time than that of a singleton consonant. It is distinct from s ...
of the central consonant of the
triliteral root The roots of verbs and most nouns in the Semitic languages are characterized as a sequence of consonants or " radicals" (hence the term consonantal root). Such abstract consonantal roots are used in the formation of actual words by adding the vowe ...
, as follows: * ''ʿalima'' "he knew" → ''ʿallama'' "he taught" The ''ʾa-'' form (Form IV), while it is used in
Modern Standard Arabic Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) or Modern Written Arabic (MWA), terms used mostly by linguists, is the variety of standardized, literary Arabic that developed in the Arab world in the late 19th and early 20th centuries; occasionally, it also ref ...
, is no longer productive in many of the colloquial
varieties of Arabic The varieties (or dialects or vernacular languages) of Arabic, a Semitic language within the Afroasiatic family originating in the Arabian Peninsula, are the linguistic systems that Arabic speakers speak natively. There are considerable variati ...
, which uniformly prefer Form II.


Japanese

Japanese Japanese may refer to: * Something from or related to Japan, an island country in East Asia * Japanese language, spoken mainly in Japan * Japanese people, the ethnic group that identifies with Japan through ancestry or culture ** Japanese diaspor ...
has lexical forms and a morphological device to signify causation. Lexical forms come in pairs of intransitive and transitive verbs, where the causee is mostly inanimate. *''ochiru'' "to fall" → ''otosu'' "to drop (something) or to let fall" However, both intransitive and transitive verbs can form the causative in a mostly regular pattern, now with the causee being mostly animate: *''hairu'' "to go in" → ''hairaseru'' "to let or force (someone) in" *''ireru'' "to put in" → ''iresaseru'' "to let of force (someone) put (something) in" In the context of an intransitive verb, the syntax of Japanese causatives allows a two-way distinction in the causee's willingness to perform the action. If the new object is marked in the accusative case (''o''), it suggests that the causee did the action willingly, suggesting the agent allowed or requested the action rather than forcing or demanding it. However, if the object is marked in the
dative case In grammar, the dative case (abbreviated , or sometimes when it is a core argument) is a grammatical case used in some languages to indicate the recipient or beneficiary of an action, as in "Maria Jacobo potum dedit", Latin for "Maria gave Jacob a ...
(''ni''), it expresses the idea that the causee was forced to perform the action. With a transitive verb, this contrast is not directly visible as a clause cannot contain two noun phrases marked as accusative.


Khmer

Khmer has six prefixes and one infix to derive the causative form of verbs, but they vary in frequency and productiveness. The consonantal prefix ''p-'' is one of them: *''coap'' "joined" → ''pcoap'' "to join" *''cum'' "around" → ''pcum'' "to gather"


Uralic languages


Finnish

Causative forms are also found in the
Uralic languages The Uralic languages (; sometimes called Uralian languages ) form a language family of 38 languages spoken by approximately 25million people, predominantly in Northern Eurasia. The Uralic languages with the most native speakers are Hungarian (w ...
of Europe, such as
Finnish Finnish may refer to: * Something or someone from, or related to Finland * Culture of Finland * Finnish people or Finns, the primary ethnic group in Finland * Finnish language, the national language of the Finnish people * Finnish cuisine See also ...
: * ''syödä'' "to eat" → ''syöttää'' "to feed" * ''täysi'' "full" → ''täyttää'' "to fill" * ''haihtua'' "to evaporate" → ''haihduttaa'' "to vaporize" The causative suffix is often used irregularly and/or because of historical reasons, as the following Finnish examples: * ''olla'' "to be" → ''olettaa'' "to assume", not "to make exist" * ''kirja-'' ancient "patterns (of embroidery or text)" but modern "book" → ''kirjoittaa'' "to write" ("transform into patterns of text"), not "to transform into books"


Hungarian

Hungarian marks the original subject of an intransitive differently in causative forms to convey direct causation. If the causee is marked by the accusative case, a more direct causation is implied than if the instrumental case is used.


Austronesian languages


Māori

In Māori, an Austronesian language, the ''whaka-'' prefix can be added to a verb: * ''ako'' "to learn" becomes ''whakaako'' "to teach" (to cause to learn)


Philippine languages

In
Philippine languages The Philippine languages or Philippinic are a proposed group by R. David Paul Zorc (1986) and Robert Blust (1991; 2005; 2019) that include all the languages of the Philippines and northern Sulawesi, Indonesia—except Sama–Bajaw (languag ...
such as Tagalog and Ilokano, the ''pa-'' prefix is added to verbal forms and to adjectives to form causatives: * ''dakkel'' "big (adjective)" → ''padakkelen'' "to enlarge" (Ilokano) * ''kain'' "eat" → ''pakainin'' "to make eat, to feed" (Tagalog)


Malay

In Malay/ Indonesian, causatives are formed from the prefix ''per-'' (it becomes ''memper-'' after actor focus/active prefix ''meng-'', expected ''*memer-'' as in ''*memerhatikan'' found informally). While most languages uses their causative affix for derivational purposes, it has integrated to Malay verb inflection system. * ''baik'' "good" → ''memperbaiki'' "to fix something" * ''tanggung jawab'' "responsibility" → ''mempertanggungjawabkan'' "to take responsibility"


Guaraní

In
Guaraní Guarani, Guaraní or Guarany may refer to Ethnography * Guaraní people, an indigenous people from South America's interior (Brazil, Argentina, Paraguay and Bolivia) * Guaraní language, or Paraguayan Guarani, an official language of Paraguay * ...
, there are three causatives: one for transitive verbs and two for intransitive verbs.Sebastian Nordhoff: Nomen/Verb-Distinktion im Guarani,
version in the internet
(downloaded 17. October 2012)
In some texts, the first one is called "coactive."Description of the language
(downloaded 19. September 2012.)
The ''-uka'' suffix (or one of its allomorphes: ''-yka'', ''-ka'') is added to transitive verbs: * ''ajapo'' "I make" → ''japouka'' "I make (someone) do". The ''mbo-'' prefix is added to intransitive oral verbs and is replaced by ''mo-'' for nasal verbs: * ''puka''Dictionary
(downloaded 19. September 2012)
"to laugh" → ''mbopuka'' "to make (someone) laugh" * ''guata'' "to walk" → ''mboguata'' "to guide" * ''pu'ã'' "to go up" → ''mopu'ã'' "to elevate" The ''guero-'' (''rero-'' or just ''ro-'') prefix can also be added to intransitive verbs. It has a
comitative In grammar, the comitative case (; abbreviated ) is a grammatical case that denotes accompaniment. In English, the preposition "with", in the sense of "in company with" or "together with", plays a substantially similar role (other uses of "with", l ...
meaning and translates roughly as "to cause something or someone to participate in an action with the subject:" * ''guata'' "to walk" → ''roguata'' "to make (someone) take a walk with (the subject)" The same root (''guata'') can take both causatives but with different meanings.


Uto-Aztecan languages


Classical Nahuatl

Classical Nahuatl, in the
Uto-Aztecan language Uto-Aztecan, Uto-Aztekan or (rarely in English) Uto-Nahuatl is a family of indigenous languages of the Americas, consisting of over thirty languages. Uto-Aztecan languages are found almost entirely in the Western United States and Mexico. The n ...
family, has a well-developed morphological system of expressing causation by means of the suffix ''-tia'': * ''tlacua'' "he eats something" → ''quitlacualtia'' "he feeds him/her/it something" the causative makes the intransitive verb "eat something" into the bitransitive verb "feed someone something," requiring a
pronominal In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun (abbreviated ) is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase. Pronouns have traditionally been regarded as one of the parts of speech, but some modern theorists would not c ...
prefix, in this case ''qui-'' "him/her/it") Causativity is often used in honorific speech in Classical Nahuatl, and rather than simply "doing," the honored person "causes himself to do."Karttunen, Frances. "Conventions of Polite Speech in Nahuatl." Estudios de Cultura Nahuatl 20(1990)


Athabaskan languages

Rice makes the following points about morphological causatives in
Athabaskan languages Athabaskan (also spelled ''Athabascan'', ''Athapaskan'' or ''Athapascan'', and also known as Dene) is a large family of indigenous languages of North America, located in western North America in three areal language groups: Northern, Pacific C ...
: *In all Athabaskan languages surveyed ncluding Hupa, for which an ample data set is presented the causativizing morphology can causativize at least some intransitive verbs with patientive subjects.Rice, Keren. 2000. "Voice and valency in the Athabaskan family." In Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds. 2000. ''Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity''. New York: Cambridge University Press. *For intransitive verbs with agentive patients, the family shows a split: only some languages then allow morphological causativization. *Koyukon (Northern Athabaskan; Alaska) was found to be the only language in the survey allowing productive morphological causativization of transitive verbs. *Perhaps the presence of the direct object pronoun in the causative construction has something to do with whether the causee is human or animate or is capable of being regarded as such. When the causee or the verb cannot be or is not perceived as a potential controller, the pronoun is not found n the Athabaskan languages surveyed The semantic factor of causee control, or the degree of control that that causee wields over the effecting of the caused microevent (also discussed as parameter #3 on Dixon's (2000:62) list) and which Rice (2001) finds to be a major factor in other Athabaskan causatives helps account for much of the distribution of the Hupa syntactic causative (below).


Hupa

Golla, in his (1970) descriptive grammar of Hupa (summarized in Sapir and Golla (2001)), describes three classes of morphologically derived causatives: While Golla does not generalize about the semantics of verb themes that are compatible with causative ƚ-, several preliminary generalizations can be made. Firstly, in the three cases described by Golla, O he undergoeris neither controlling nor agentive; O is largely patientive in all cases. Secondly, the causer appears to be acting directly on O. Thirdly, none of the examples given (including the examples above) involve the causativization of a base-transitive theme.


Central Alaskan Yup'ik

Mithun (2000) lists nine causatives for Central Alaskan Yup'ik and describes each in detail. Here is a brief description of each:


Bantu languages


Kinyarwanda

Kinyarwanda Kinyarwanda, Rwandan or Rwanda, officially known as Ikinyarwanda, is a Bantu language and a dialect of the Rwanda-Rundi language that is spoken in Rwanda and adjacent parts of Burundi, the Democratic Republic of the Congo, Uganda (where t ...
uses periphrastic causatives and morphological causatives. The periphrastic causatives use the verbs ''-teer-'' and ''-tum-'', which mean ''cause''. With ''-teer-'', the original S becomes the O of the main clause, leaving the original verb in the infinitive, just like in English:Kinyarwanda: Kimenyi, Alexandre (1980). ''A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda''. University of California Press. p. 160–72. With ''-túm-'', the original S remains in the embedded clause and the original verb is still marked for person and tense: Derivational causatives use the ''-iish-'' morpheme, which can be applied to intransitives (3) or transitives (4): The suffix ''-iish-'' implies an indirect causation (similar to English ''have'' in "I had him write a paper"), but other causatives imply a direct causation (similar to English ''make'' in "I made him write a paper"). One of the more direct causation devices is the deletion of what is called a "neutral" morpheme ''-ik-'', which indicates state or potentiality. Stems with the ''-ik-'' removed can take ''-iish'', but the causation is then less direct: : Another direct causation maker is ''-y-'' which is used for some verbs:


Esperanto

In Esperanto, the suffix ''-ig-'' can be added to any kind of word: * ''morti'' "to die" → ''mortigi'' "to kill" * ''pura'' "clean (adj)" → ''purigi'' "to clean"


References


Further reading


General reading

*Croft, W. 2003. Typology and Universals, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. *Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald. 2000. “Introduction”. In ''Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity'', Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds: 1–28. New York: Cambridge University Press. *Goertz, G. et al. 2006. “Use of causatives in Navajo: Syntax and morphology.” In Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics, Volume 18: Proceedings from the Ninth Workshop on American Indigenous Languages. *Huang, S. and Lily I-Wen Su. 2005. “Iconicity as Evidenced in Saisiyat Linguistic Coding of Causative Events.” Oceanic Linguistics, Vol. 44, No. 2 (Dec., 2005): 341–356. *Song, J.J. (2001) Linguistic Typology: Morphology and Syntax. Harlow and London: Pearson (Longman). *Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 1: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.


Causatives of a specific language

*Kinyarwanda: Kimenyi, Alexandre (1980). ''A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda''. University of California Press. p. 160–72.


External links


What is a causative?
{{Grammatical cases Linguistic morphology Syntactic categories Transitivity and valency Grammatical cases