Examples
Take the sentence ''John helped Bill in Central Park on Sunday'' as an example: :# ''John'' is the subject argument. :# ''helped'' is the predicate. :# ''Bill'' is the object argument. :# ''in Central Park'' is the first adjunct. :# ''on Sunday'' is the second adjunct. An ''adverbial adjunct'' is a sentence element that often establishes the circumstances in which the action or state expressed by theForms and domains
An adjunct can be a single word, aSemantic function
Adjuncts can be categorized in terms of the functional meaning that they contribute to the phrase, clause, or sentence in which they appear. The following list of the semantic functions is by no means exhaustive, but it does include most of the semantic functions of adjuncts identified in the literature on adjuncts: ::Causal – Causal adjuncts establish the reason for, or purpose of, an action or state. ::The ladder collapsed because it was old. (reason) ::Concessive – Concessive adjuncts establish contrary circumstances. ::Lorna went out although it was raining. ::Conditional – Conditional adjuncts establish the condition in which an action occurs or state holds. ::I would go to Paris, if I had the money. ::Consecutive – Consecutive adjuncts establish an effect or result. ::It rained so hard that the streets flooded. ::Final – Final adjuncts establish the goal of an action (what one wants to accomplish). ::He works a lot to earn money for school. ::Instrumental – Instrumental adjuncts establish the instrument used to accomplish an action. ::Mr. Bibby wrote the letter with a pencil. ::Locative – Locative adjuncts establish where, to where, or from where a state or action happened or existed. ::She sat on the table. (locative) ::Measure – Measure adjuncts establish the measure of the action, state, or quality that they modify ::I am completely finished. ::That is mostly true. ::We want to stay in part. ::Modal – Modal adjuncts establish the extent to which the speaker views the action or state as (im)probable. ::They probably left. ::In any case, we didn't do it. ::That is perhaps possible. ::I'm definitely going to the party. ::Modificative – Modificative adjuncts establish how the action happened or the state existed. ::He ran with difficulty. (manner) ::He stood in silence. (state) ::He helped me with my homework. (limiting) ::Temporal – Temporal adjuncts establish when, how long, or how frequent the action or state happened or existed. ::He arrived yesterday. (time point) ::He stayed for two weeks. (duration) ::She drinks in that bar every day. (frequency)Distinguishing between predicative expressions, arguments, and adjuncts
Omission diagnostic
The distinction between arguments and adjuncts and predicates is central to most theories of syntax and grammar. Predicates take arguments and they permit (certain) adjuncts. The arguments of a predicate are necessary to complete the meaning of the predicate. The adjuncts of a predicate, in contrast, provide auxiliary information about the core predicate-argument meaning, which means they are not necessary to complete the meaning of the predicate. Adjuncts and arguments can be identified using various diagnostics. The omission diagnostic, for instance, helps identify many arguments and thus indirectly many adjuncts as well. If a given constituent cannot be omitted from a sentence, clause, or phrase without resulting in an unacceptable expression, that constituent is NOT an adjunct, e.g. ::a. Fred certainly knows. ::b. Fred knows. – ''certainly'' may be an adjunct (and it is). ::a. He stayed after class. ::b. He stayed. – ''after class'' may be an adjunct (and it is). ::a. She trimmed the bushes. ::b. *She trimmed. – ''the bushes'' is NOT an adjunct. ::a. Jim stopped. ::b. *Stopped. – ''Jim'' is NOT an adjunct.Other diagnostics
Further diagnostics used to distinguish between arguments and adjuncts include multiplicity, distance from head, and the ability to coordinate. A head can have multiple adjuncts but only one object argument (=complement): ::a. Bob ate the pizza. – ''the pizza'' is an object argument (=complement). ::b. Bob ate the pizza and the hamburger. ''the pizza and the hamburger'' is a noun phrase that functions as object argument. ::c. Bob ate the pizza with a fork. – ''with a fork'' is an adjunct. ::d. Bob ate the pizza with a fork on Tuesday. – ''with a fork'' and ''on Tuesday'' are both adjuncts. Object arguments are typically closer to their head than adjuncts: ::a. the collection of figurines (complement) in the dining room (adjunct) ::b. *the collection in the dining room (adjunct) of figurines (complement) Adjuncts can be coordinated with other adjuncts, but not with arguments: ::a. *Bob ate the pizza and with a fork. ::b. Bob ate with a fork and with a spoon.Optional arguments vs. adjuncts
The distinction between arguments and adjuncts is much less clear than the simple omission diagnostic (and the other diagnostics) suggests. Most accounts of the argument vs. adjunct distinction acknowledge a further division. One distinguishes between obligatory and optional arguments. Optional arguments pattern like adjuncts when just the omission diagnostic is employed, e.g. ::a. Fred ate a hamburger. ::b. Fred ate. – ''a hamburger'' is NOT an obligatory argument, but it could be (and it is) an optional argument. ::a. Sam helped us. ::b. Sam helped – ''us'' is NOT an obligatory argument, but it could be (and it is) an optional argument. The existence of optional arguments blurs the line between arguments and adjuncts considerably. Further diagnostics (beyond the omission diagnostic and the others mentioned above) must be employed to distinguish between adjuncts and optional arguments. One such diagnostic is the relative clause test. The test constituent is moved from the matrix clause to a subordinate relative clause containing ''which occurred/happened''. If the result is unacceptable, the test constituent is probably NOT an adjunct: ::a. Fred ate a hamburger. ::b. Fred ate. – ''a hamburger'' is not an obligatory argument. ::c. *Fred ate, which occurred a hamburger. – ''a hamburger'' is not an adjunct, which means it must be an optional argument. ::a. Sam helped us. ::b. Sam helped. – ''us'' is not an obligatory argument. ::c. *Sam helped, which occurred us. – ''us'' is not an adjunct, which means it must be an optional argument. The particular merit of the relative clause test is its ability to distinguish between many argument and adjunct PPs, e.g. ::a. We are working on the problem. ::b. We are working. ::c. *We are working, which is occurring on the problem. – ''on the problem'' is an optional argument. ::a. They spoke to the class. ::b. They spoke. ::c. *They spoke, which occurred to the class. – ''to the class'' is an optional argument. The reliability of the relative clause diagnostic is actually limited. For instance, it incorrectly suggests that many modal and manner adjuncts are arguments. This fact bears witness to the difficulty of providing an absolute diagnostic for the distinctions currently being examined. Despite the difficulties, most theories of syntax and grammar distinguish on the one hand between arguments and adjuncts and on the other hand between optional arguments and adjuncts, and they grant a central position to these divisions in the overarching theory.Predicates vs. adjuncts
Many phrases have the outward appearance of an adjunct but are in fact (part of) a predicate instead. The confusion occurs often with copular verbs, in particular with a form of ''be'', e.g. ::It is under the bush. ::The party is at seven o'clock. The PPs in these sentences are NOT adjuncts, nor are they arguments. The preposition in each case is, rather, part of the main predicate. The matrix predicate in the first sentence is ''is under''; this predicate takes the two arguments ''It'' and ''the bush''. Similarly, the matrix predicate in the second sentence is ''is at''; this predicate takes the two arguments ''The party'' and ''seven o'clock''. Distinguishing between predicates, arguments, and adjuncts becomes particularly difficult when secondary predicates are involved, for instance with resultative predicates, e.g. ::That made him tired. The resultative adjective ''tired'' can be viewed as an argument of the matrix predicate ''made''. But it is also definitely a predicate over ''him''. Such examples illustrate that distinguishing predicates, arguments, and adjuncts can become difficult and there are many cases where a given expression functions in more ways than one.Overview
The following overview is a breakdown of the current divisions: ::Representing adjuncts
Many theories of syntax and grammar employ trees to represent the structure of sentences. Various conventions are used to distinguish between arguments and adjuncts in these trees. InSee also
*Adverbial *Argument (linguistics), Argument *Conjunct *Disjunct (linguistics), Disjunct *Noun adjunct *Predicate (grammar), Predicate *Predicative expression *AttributiveNotes
References
{{refbegin, 2 *Eroms, H.-W. 2000. Syntax der deutschen Sprache. Berlin: de Gruyter. *Carnie, A. 2010. ''Constituent Structure.'' Oxford: Oxford U.P. *Lyons J. 1968. Introduction to theoretical linguistics. London: Cambridge U.P. *Payne, T. 2006. Exploring language structure: A student's guide. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. *Tesnière, L. 1959. Éleménts de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck. Syntactic entities es:Complemento circunstancial