Vamale Language
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Vamale (Pamale) is a Kanak language of northern
New Caledonia ) , anthem = "" , image_map = New Caledonia on the globe (small islands magnified) (Polynesia centered).svg , map_alt = Location of New Caledonia , map_caption = Location of New Caledonia , mapsize = 290px , subdivision_type = Sovereign st ...
. The Hmwaeke dialect, spoken in Tiéta, is fusing with Haveke and nearly extinct. Vamale is nowadays spoken in Tiendanite (called "Usa Vamale"), We Hava, Téganpaïk and Tiouandé. It was spoken in the Pamale valley and its tributaries Vawe and Usa until the colonial of war of 1917, when its speakers were displaced.


Phonology

Vamale has five phonemic vowel articulations, and 35 consonant phonemes.


Vowels

While Vamale distinguishes five vowel phonemes, /a/, /e/, /i/, /o/, /u/, nasality and
length Length is a measure of distance. In the International System of Quantities, length is a quantity with dimension distance. In most systems of measurement a base unit for length is chosen, from which all other units are derived. In the Interna ...
are phonemic as well. Compare /tã/ 'oven' and /ta/ 'go up', /ˈfa.ti/ 'speech' and /ˈfaː.ti/ 'to stick, to glue'. Depending on the length of the vowel, and on the final consonant of the syllable, /e/ and /o/ can be realized more open: plosives and short syllables trigger open vowels (e.g. ɔːt'grass' ɛn'poison'), while open syllables, and long ones closed by nasals, feature closed vowels.


Consonants

As is typical of Northern New Caledonian languages, Vamale has a wealth of consonants. The distinction reconstructible since
Proto-Oceanic Proto-Oceanic (abbr. ''POc'') is a proto-language that historical linguists since Otto Dempwolff have reconstructed as the hypothetical common ancestor of the Oceanic subgroup of the Austronesian language family. Proto-Oceanic is a descendant ...
is between nasals, semi-nasals (pre-nasalized voiced plosives, i.e. /ᵐb/, /ⁿɟ/ etc.), and orals.


Stress

In Vamale, disyllabic words have penultimate stress, as is typical in Oceanic settings. Trisyllabic, morphologically simple, non-derived nouns take stress on the first syllable. These are rare, though loanwords now increase their number. Longer words are morphologically complex and put stress on the penultimate stress-bearing unit, which is often a syllable of the root, but not always. While some morphological factors complicate the picture, regular phonological aspects predict most stress positions. A closed syllable will be stressed over an open one, a fortis onset will usually top a tenuis onset, and a long syllable will be stressed above all else. This gives us a hierarchy of factors: Long syllable > fortis onset > closed syllable > penultimate syllable    Some monosyllabic morphemes do not count in the stress pattern. One frequent example is the extrametrical suffix ''-ke'' 'TR', whose phonological non-importance makes the third syllable in /fʷan.ˈɟi.mʷa.ke/ 'ask something' the penultimate of the phonological word. ''-ke'' may be related to the proclitic ''ka'' which marks subjects and possessors.    Possessive and object-indexing suffixes shift the stress, but not in a simple syllable-counting way, as ᵐbwãn.ɟɛp'bark clapper (a type of hand drum', bwãn.ˈɟɛp.go'hand drum, hand drum-2G.POSS' would suggest, i.e. with the stress, all things being equal, moving to the new penultimate syllable. However, since ''bwanjep-gavwe'' ᵐbwãn.ˈɟɛp.ga.vʷe'hand drum-2PL.POSS' does not have the stress on the penultimate syllable of the phonological word, a this suggests an analysis of root syllables that is different from that of suffixed morphology. Possessive and object-indexing suffixes count as a single unit in stress assignment, meaning that a two-syllable possessive suffix such as ''-gavwe'' '2SG.POSS (your)' has the same effect as -''go'' '2SG.POSS, your (singular)'. Speech act participant indexes (the proclitics, not the suffixes) are also extrametrical:   /ˈɣa.le.ke/ 'to see'    le=ˈɣa.le-le/ '3PL=see-3PL.OBJ', 'they see them'    /le=ɣa.le-ˈkaː.vʷe/ 'they see you', also pronounced /le=ɣa.ˈle.ka.vʷe/ Other syllables attract stress. The nominalizer ''xa-'' 'AGT.NMLZ' (from ''xayu'' 'male') always attracts stress (probably due to its etymology, but not ''hun''- 'manner.NMLZ' nor ''ape-'' 'place.NMLZ' (from ''ape-'' 'trace'). Semantically bleached function words like /a.ˈman/ 'thing; object place-holder' are re-analyzed as one
foot The foot ( : feet) is an anatomical structure found in many vertebrates. It is the terminal portion of a limb which bears weight and allows locomotion. In many animals with feet, the foot is a separate organ at the terminal part of the leg made ...
in compounds: tɕaj.n̥ãn'know', ɕaj.ˈn̥ãn.ã.mãn'know something' ɕãm.bi'smash', .tɕãm.ˈbi.jã.mãn 'hammer' The complex word ''ape-caihnan-aman-le'', 'NMLZ-know-thing-3PL.POSS' 'their knowledge', is pronounced a.pe.tɕaj.n̥ãn.ã.ˈmãn.le which could be explained by analyzing ''(ape-)caihnan-aman'' '(fact.of-)know-thing' as a compound, ''-le'' 'their' as a suffix, and thus the main stress on the penultimate syllable. For Hmwaveke, stress is described as being fundamentally penultimate, and forms which deviate from this, with few exceptions, are analyzed by Campbell as several phonological words. Campbell analyzes long syllables in plurisyllabic words as resulting from stress (suggesting that, fundamentally, length is a feature of all stressed syllables). For Vamale, though long syllables are stressed, we argue that the relation is reversedː length attracts stress. In Nêlêmwa, stress is usually on the first syllable of the lexical root ''kâ-ˈyuva'' 'how? (lying.down-be.thus)'. This correlation between morphological makeup and stress pattern is mirrored in Vamale to a certain extent, in that bound morphemes such as ''e-'' ' RECP', ''-ke'' 'TR', and manner prefixes like ''mi''- 'do lying down' do not affect the position of the main stress.


Syntax

Like many Oceanic languages, Vamale has no
adjectives In linguistics, an adjective (abbreviated ) is a word that generally modifies a noun or noun phrase or describes its referent. Its semantic role is to change information given by the noun. Traditionally, adjectives were considered one of the mai ...
, using instead
stative verb According to some linguistics theories, a stative verb is a verb that describes a state of being, in contrast to a dynamic verb, which describes an action. The difference can be categorized by saying that stative verbs describe situations that are ...
s to express most state-like meanings. Besides a wealth of small classes of function words, the biggest classes in Vamale are nominal, including pronouns, and verbal.


Pronouns


Nouns

Nouns can be classified along several axes: alienability determines whether a noun must show possessive morphology or not, and directness shows whether possession is marked via a suffix on the noun stem, or via e.g. a possessive classifier.


Possession


Classifiers

Vamale has few classifiers, mostly concerned with the nature of food and drink. Two words distinguish drinks: ''udoo-'' 'cold drink' and ''fatoo-'' 'warm drink'. Food items are classified as follows: ''u-'' 'juicy food' ''xhua-'' 'proteiny food', ''ya-'' 'starchy food', with some rarer words for chewy foods such as roots (''fwaa-)'', and crunchy ones such as sugarcane (''xhuta-'').


Aspect

Vamale marks tempus only coincidentally, but shows prolific use of aspect markers. Aspect in Kanak languages is expressed with morphemes which often combine aspectual, modal, and temporal meanings. Overall, Vamale aspect closely resembles its counterparts in its northern sisters Nêlêmwa, Caac, and Bwatoo. The differences between them, however, may shed some light on the development of aspectual systems in the region, and provide evidence for a distinction between old and new forms. This in turn could tell us which meanings were expressed first, and if new ones were borrowed, where they came from. ''Balan'' is interesting because while it can be used alone, it is used with many other aspectual markers, its meaning changing every time. - Wordhood: Aspectual markers (Bril calls them „morphemes“, as does Cauchard) are the only elements that can be inserted between a person marker and a verb but cannot separate an article from the nominal predicate it modifies. This raises the question whether they are best seen as particles, clitics, or affixes. This view argues for a particle status, on the basis of syntactic arguments. - Position: Even though most markers occur between the person marker and the predicate (nominal or verbal), repetitive ''mwa'' is a post-predicate particle. - Combinability: Combinations are possible, in some cases preferred to the lone form (especially with ''ja'' and ''balan''), and may be non-compositional. They only occur in the canonical order (i.e. between the person marker and the predicate), which increases the appeal of a syntactic analysis of such elements. - Function: The meaning of aspectual markers depends on their position and the ''aktionsart'' of the verb (the latter of which can be changed by context and dedicated morphemes). Whether ''bwa'' ‚IPFV‘ occurs before or after the person marker subtly distinguishes between related meanings, as does the order of markers in combinations. This flexibility is not common to all markers, however.


Alignment

In New Caledonia, northern languages tend towards ergativity, and southern ones to accusativity. Vamale, on the border between the two, is split-transitive and tripartite in its verb-indexing, nominative-accusative in its subject marking on noun phrases, and has ergative patterns in nominalized verbs. Verbs in Vamale, like in Proto-Oceanic, can be classified as either “active”, with subject indexing preceding the verb, or “stative”, with subject indexing suffixed to the stem. Nominal predicates follow the active pattern, and all transitive verbs are active. While class membership is lexically determined following semantic tendencies, some derivation may take place (always from stative to active or active to active). This is a split-transitive system, and is further complicated by the fact that objects are indexed as suffixes, much like stative subjects, but do not use identical forms. The forms seem derived from former free-standing forms, which still exist, but can only co-occur with the subject index (not the object ones). They show nominative-accusative alignment regardless of the verb’s class. However, while subject and object indexing on verbs has a split-transitive, tripartite alignment, deverbal nominalizations show a more ergative alignment: undergoers and intransitive subjects are indexed alike, while transitive subjects are distinct. In Vamale, as in many other Oceanic languages, whole verb phrases, with dependent verbs and objects, can be nominalized by dropping the pre-verbal subject index and adding an article instead. Often, a nominalizer is added as well. ''le=saxhuti      i= un-thake                          hanu    ka=yo' 3PL=narrate   ART.SG=NMLZ-throw          picture SUBJ=1SG ‘They tell the story of my playing cards.’ Free pronouns are marked as subjects with a proclitic ''ka''. Another ''ka'' is a linker that shows a special possessive relationship e.g. to a chief (''daahma ka-m'' ‘chief LNK-2SG.POSS’), or between medicine and ailment (''udee ka-n nyaabu'' ‘medicine LNK-NSPEC mosquito’). If the possessor is anaphoric information (“their chief”), or the relationship a generic one (“mosquito repellent”), this is indexed on the linker via possessive morphology (instead of using the whole noun phrase), not with free pronouns. Note that there is also an animacy distinction: specific animate entities are always indexed with possessive morphology. This is the linker’s main morphosyntactic distinction from the subject-marking proclitic, which cannot take affixes. Nominalized intransitive verbs, and nominalized transitive verbs where the subject is not expressed via a free form, share the same indexing morphology. Due to the homophonous relationship of the linker ''ka'' and the subject marker ''ka'', a nominalized transitive construction featuring both an undergoer NP and a subject NP will favour the subject marker and omit the linker. Ignoring this preference is not ungrammatical per se, but labelled as “confusing”, since the resulting construction may end up looking like this: V ''ka'' O ''ka'' A.


References


External links


ELAR archive deposit containing recordings and other resources (dictionary, grammar)

CREM archive deposit including poetry and songs in Vamale
New Caledonian languages Languages of New Caledonia {{NewCaledonia-stub