Numbami Language
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Numbami (also known as Siboma or Sipoma) is an Austronesian language spoken by about 200 people with ties to a single village in
Morobe Province Morobe Province is a province on the northern coast of Papua New Guinea. The provincial capital and largest city is Lae. The province covers 33,705 km2, with a population of 674,810 (2011 census), and since the division of Southern Highlands P ...
,
Papua New Guinea Papua New Guinea (abbreviated PNG; , ; tpi, Papua Niugini; ho, Papua Niu Gini), officially the Independent State of Papua New Guinea ( tpi, Independen Stet bilong Papua Niugini; ho, Independen Stet bilong Papua Niu Gini), is a country i ...
. It is spoken in Siboma village (), Paiawa ward, Morobe Rural LLG.English-Numbami dictionary
/ref> Numbami is a phonologically conservative isolate within the Huon Gulf languages, and is the last Austronesian language on the south coast of the Huon Gulf. Its nearest relatives along the coast to the southeast are 270 km away, Maisin and Arifama-Miniafia in
Oro Province Oro Province, formerly (and officially still) Northern Province, is a coastal province of Papua New Guinea. The provincial capital is Popondetta. The province covers 22,800 km2, and has 176,206 inhabitants (2011 census). The province shares la ...
(Northern Province in the former colony of Papua). The
word order In linguistics, word order (also known as linear order) is the order of the syntactic constituents of a language. Word order typology studies it from a cross-linguistic perspective, and examines how different languages employ different orders. C ...
typology of Numbami and the Huon Gulf languages is subject–verb–object (SVO), which is typical of Austronesian languages; while that of Arifama-Miniafia and most of the Papuan Tip languages is
subject–object–verb Subject ( la, subiectus "lying beneath") may refer to: Philosophy *'' Hypokeimenon'', or ''subiectum'', in metaphysics, the "internal", non-objective being of a thing **Subject (philosophy), a being that has subjective experiences, subjective con ...
(SOV), which is typical of Papuan languages. Maisin has been characterized as a
mixed language A mixed language is a language that arises among a bilingual group combining aspects of two or more languages but not clearly deriving primarily from any single language. It differs from a creole language, creole or pidgin, pidgin language in that ...
, with both Austronesian and Papuan features that obscure its primary heritage, and there is evidence that settlements of Austronesian speakers along the 270 km of coast were gradually absorbed into inland communities speaking
Binanderean languages The Greater Binanderean or Guhu-Oro languages are a language family spoken along the northeast coast of the Papuan Peninsula – the "Bird's Tail" of New Guinea – and appear to be a recent expansion from the north. They were classified as a bra ...
(Bradshaw 2017).


Phonology

Numbami distinguishes 5 vowels and 18 consonants. Voiceless /s/ is a fricative, but its voiced and prenasalized equivalents are affricated, varying between more alveolar and more palatalized . The liquid /l/ is usually rendered as a flap . The labial approximant is slightly fricative, tending toward , when followed by front vowels.


Vowels (orthographic)


Consonants (orthographic)


Obstruent harmony

Prenasalized obstruents only occur in medial position, where the distinction between oral and prenasalized voiced obstruents is somewhat predictable. Medial voiced obstruents are statistically far more likely to be oral in words beginning with oral voiced obstruents, while they are far more likely to be prenasalized in words beginning with anything else. If denasalization of voiced obstruents is an ongoing change, one can track its progress through different lexical environments: it is 100% complete in word-initial position (as in 'pig' and 'areca nut'), 80% complete in the middle of words beginning with voiced obstruents (as in 'market' and 'head' vs. 'driftwood'), 35% complete in the middle of words beginning with approximants or vowels (as in 'crosswise' vs. 'handdrum' and 'canoe'), not quite 20% complete in words beginning with voiceless obstruents (as in 'left side' vs. 'butterfly' and 'flea'), not quite 5% complete in words beginning with nasals (as in 'thing' and 'to die'), and not attested at all in words beginning with liquids (as in 'nit' and 'lime spatula'). (See Bradshaw 1978a.)


Morphology

Although Numbami is phonologically conservative, it retains very little productive morphology, most of it related to person and number marking.


Pronouns and person markers


Free pronouns

Free pronouns occur in the same positions as subject or object nouns. They distinguish three persons (with a clusivity distinction in the first person) and four numbers (Bradshaw 1982a).


Genitive pronouns

Genitive pronouns also distinguish three persons (plus clusivity) and four numbers (Bradshaw 1982a).


Subject prefixes

Verbs are marked with subject prefixes that distinguish three persons (plus clusivity) and two tenses, Nonfuture and Future. (The latter distinction is often characterized as one between Realis and Irrealis mode; see Bradshaw 1993, 1999.) In most cases, subject prefixes are easily segmentable from verb stems, but in a few very high frequency cases, prefix-final vowels merge with verb-initial vowels to yield irregularly inflected forms, as in the following paradigm: ''wani'' (< ''wa-ani'') '1SG-eat', ''woni'' (< ''u-ani'') '2SG-eat', ''weni'' (< ''i-ani'') '3SG-eat', ''tani'' (< ''ta-ani'') '1PLINCL-eat', ''mani'' (< ''ma-ani'') '1PLEXCL-eat', ''moni'' (< ''mu-ani'') '2PL-eat', ''teni'' (< ''ti-ani'') '3PL-eat'.


Numerals

Traditional Numbami counting practices started with the digits of the left hand, then continued on the right hand and then the feet, to reach '20', which translates as 'one person'. Higher numbers are multiples of 'one person'. Nowadays, most counting above '5' is done in Tok Pisin. As in other Huon Gulf languages, the short form of the numeral 'one' functions as an indefinite article.


Names

Like many other Huon Gulf languages, Numbami has a system of birth-order names. The seventh son and sixth daughter are called "No Name": Ase Mou 'name none'.


Ideophones

Although many languages have a class of
ideophone Ideophone is a word class evoking ideas in sound imitation or onomatopoeia to express action, manner of property. Ideophone is the least common syntactic category cross-linguistically occurring mostly in African, Australian and Amerindian langua ...
s with distinctive phonology, Numbami is unusual in having a morphological marker for such a class. The suffix ''-a(n)dala'' is unique to ideophones but is clearly related to the word ''andalowa'' 'path, way, road' (POc *jalan). (See Bradshaw 2006.) In the following examples, acute accents show the placement of word stress. * ''bái-andala'' 'overcast, clouded over' * ''dendende-ándala'' 'shivering' * ''golópu-adala'' 'slipping or dripping through' * ''kí-andala'' 'scorching, parched' * ''paká-adala'' 'getting light, flashing on, popping' * ''pilipíli-adala'' 'flapping, fluttering' * ''sí-andala'' 'shooting up, springing away' * ''sulúku-adala'' 'sucking, slurping' * ''taká-adala'' 'stuck fast, planted firmly' * ''tíki-adala'' 'going dark'


Syntax


Word order

The basic
word order In linguistics, word order (also known as linear order) is the order of the syntactic constituents of a language. Word order typology studies it from a cross-linguistic perspective, and examines how different languages employ different orders. C ...
in Numbami is SVO, with prepositions, preposed genitives, postposed adjectives and relative clauses.
Relative clause A relative clause is a clause that modifies a noun or noun phraseRodney D. Huddleston, Geoffrey K. Pullum, ''A Student's Introduction to English Grammar'', CUP 2005, p. 183ff. and uses some grammatical device to indicate that one of the arguments ...
s are marked at both ends, and so are some
prepositional phrase An adpositional phrase, in linguistics, is a syntactic category that includes ''prepositional phrases'', ''postpositional phrases'', and ''circumpositional phrases''. Adpositional phrases contain an adposition (preposition, postposition, or circ ...
s. Negatives come at the ends of the clauses they negate. There is also a class of deverbal resultatives that follow the main verb (and its object, if any). :'The two of them killed a pig.' :'My cross-cousin doesn't understand Numbami.' :'We (excl.) put the food we've pared into the pot.'


Possessive vs. attributive genitives

Two kinds of genitive modifiers precede their heads while one type follows its head noun (Bradshaw 1982a).


Whole-part genitives

Noun-noun phrases denoting wholes and parts occur in the order stated, with the latter serving as head of the phrase: ''wuwu lau'' 'betel pepper leaf', ''tina daba'' 'headwater', ''nima daba'' (lit. 'hand head') 'thumb', ''kapala lalo'' (lit. 'house inside') 'indoors', ''Buzina bubusu'' 'Buzina (Salamaua) point'.


Possessive genitives

Genitive possessor nouns precede their head nouns, with an intervening possessive marker that distinguishes singular (''na'') from plural (''ndi'') possessors: ''wuwu na lau'' 'the leaves of the (generic) betel pepper plant; particular betel pepper plant's leaf'; ''kapala na lalo'' 'the insides of (generic) houses; the inside of a particular house'; ''Siasi ndi gutu'' 'the Siassi Islands; islands belonging to a particular group of Siassi people'; ''bumewe ndi bani'' 'food typically eaten by whites; food belonging to a particular group of whites'.


Attributive genitives

Attributive genitives resemble possessive genitives except that (1) the modifiers follow their heads, and (2) the "possessors" are nonreferential except in a generic sense, that is, they "never refer to a particular subset of the set they name" (Bradshaw 1982a:128): ''wuwu weni na'' 'forest (wild) betel pepper', ''wuwu Buzina ndi'' 'type of betel pepper associated with the Buzina people at Salamaua', ''walabeŋa tamtamoŋa na'' 'fish poison, native means of stunning fish', ''walabeŋa bumewe na'' 'explosives, European means of stunning fish'.


Verb serialization

Verb serialization The serial verb construction, also known as (verb) serialization or verb stacking, is a syntactic phenomenon in which two or more verbs or verb phrases are strung together in a single clause.Tallerman, M. (1998). ''Understanding Syntax''. London: ...
is very common in Numbami. Within a serial verb construction, all verbs must agree in tense.
Subject Subject ( la, subiectus "lying beneath") may refer to: Philosophy *''Hypokeimenon'', or ''subiectum'', in metaphysics, the "internal", non-objective being of a thing **Subject (philosophy), a being that has subjective experiences, subjective cons ...
choice in successive verbs is severely constrained. Noninitial subjects can only refer to preceding subjects, preceding objects, or preceding events or conditions, and only in that order (Bradshaw 1993). Negatives come at the ends of the clauses they negate. :'Okay, we come back down carrying food.' :'Don't you turn around and talk to me.' :'They'll put dry fronds on top of the sago starch.' :'And you'll set it afire.' :'I saw you first.' :'Nowadays, we two don't see each other very regularly.'


External links


Numbami (Siboma) Wordlist at the Austronesian Basic Vocabulary Database

Numbami-English dictionary (PDF)

English-Numbami dictionary (PDF)
* Recorded and written materials for Numbami are archived with
Kaipuleohone Kaipuleohone is a digital ethnographic archive that houses audio and visual files, photographs, as well as hundreds of textual material such as notes, dictionaries, and transcriptions relating to small and endangered languages. The archive is stored ...


References

* Bradshaw, Joel (2017). Evidence of contact between Binanderean and Oceanic languages. ''Oceanic Linguistics'' 56:395–414. * Bradshaw, Joel (2010). Manner and path verb serialization in Numbami. In ''A journey through Austronesian and Papuan linguistic and cultural space: Papers in honour of Andrew Pawley,'' ed. by John Bowden, Nikolaus P Himmelmann, and Malcolm Ross, pp. 441–453. Pacific Linguistics 615. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. * Bradshaw, Joel (2006). Grammatically marked ideophones in Numbami and Jabêm. ''Oceanic Linguistics'' 45:53–63. * Bradshaw, Joel (1999). Null subjects, switch-reference, and serialization in Jabêm and Numbami. ''Oceanic Linguistics'' 38:270–296. * Bradshaw, Joel (1997). The population kaleidoscope: Another factor in the Melanesian diversity v. Polynesian homogeneity debate. ''Journal of the Polynesian Society'' 106:222–249. * Bradshaw, Joel (1993). Subject relationships within serial verb constructions in Numbami and Jabêm. ''Oceanic Linguistics'' 32:133–161. * Bradshaw, Joel (1982a). Genitives and relatives in Numbami, a New Guinea Austronesian language. In ''Gava‘: Studies in Austronesian languages and cultures dedicated to Hans Kähler,'' ed. by Rainer Carle, Martina Heinschke, Peter W. Pink, Christel Rost, and Karen Stadtlander, pp. 123–139. Veröffentlichungen des Seminars für Indonesische und Südseesprachen der Universität Hamburg, vol. 17. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer. * Bradshaw, Joel (1982b)
Word order change in Papua New Guinea Austronesian languages
Ph.D. dissertation, University of Hawaii. * Bradshaw, Joel (1978a). The development of an extra series of obstruents in Numbami. ''Oceanic Linguistics'' 17:39–76. * Bradshaw, Joel (1978b)
Multilingualism and language mixture among the Numbami
''Kivung: Journal of the Linguistic Society of Papua New Guinea'' 11:26-49. * Ross, Malcolm (1988). ''Proto Oceanic and the Austronesian languages of western Melanesia.'' Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. {{Languages of Papua New Guinea North New Guinea languages Languages of Morobe Province Subject–verb–object languages Mixed languages Vulnerable languages