Motion Aftereffect
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

The motion aftereffect (MAE) is a
visual illusion Within visual perception, an optical illusion (also called a visual illusion) is an illusion caused by the visual system and characterized by a visual percept that arguably appears to differ from reality. Illusions come in a wide variety; thei ...
experienced after viewing a moving visual
stimulus A stimulus is something that causes a physiological response. It may refer to: *Stimulation **Stimulus (physiology), something external that influences an activity **Stimulus (psychology), a concept in behaviorism and perception *Stimulus (economi ...
for a time (tens of milliseconds to minutes) with stationary eyes, and then fixating a stationary stimulus. The stationary stimulus appears to move in the opposite direction to the original (physically moving) stimulus. The motion aftereffect is believed to be the result of
motion In physics, motion is the phenomenon in which an object changes its position with respect to time. Motion is mathematically described in terms of displacement, distance, velocity, acceleration, speed and frame of reference to an observer and mea ...
adaptation In biology, adaptation has three related meanings. Firstly, it is the dynamic evolutionary process of natural selection that fits organisms to their environment, enhancing their evolutionary fitness. Secondly, it is a state reached by the po ...
. For example, if one looks at a waterfall for about a minute and then looks at the stationary rocks at the side of the waterfall, these rocks appear to be moving upwards slightly. The illusory upwards movement is the motion aftereffect. This particular motion aftereffect is also known as the ''waterfall illusion''. Another example can be seen when one looks at the center of a rotating spiral for several seconds. The spiral can exhibit outward or inward motion. When one then looks at any stationary pattern, it appears to be moving in the opposite direction. This form of the motion aftereffect is known as the ''spiral aftereffect''.


Explanation

Neuron A neuron, neurone, or nerve cell is an electrically excitable cell that communicates with other cells via specialized connections called synapses. The neuron is the main component of nervous tissue in all animals except sponges and placozoa. N ...
s coding a particular movement reduce their responses with time of exposure to a constantly moving stimulus; this is
neural adaptation Neural adaptation or sensory adaptation is a gradual decrease over time in the responsiveness of the sensory system to a constant stimulus. It is usually experienced as a change in the stimulus. For example, if a hand is rested on a table, the ta ...
. Neural adaptation also reduces the spontaneous, baseline activity of these same neurons when responding to a stationary stimulus (see, for example, Barlow & Hill, 1963; Srinivasan & Dvorak, 1979; Glasser, Tsui, Pack, & Tadin, 2011). One theory is that perception of stationary objects, for example rocks beside a waterfall, is coded as the balance among the baseline responses of neurons coding all possible directions of motion. Neural adaptation of neurons stimulated by downward movement reduces their baseline activity, tilting the balance in favor of upward movement.


History

Aristotle Aristotle (; grc-gre, Ἀριστοτέλης ''Aristotélēs'', ; 384–322 BC) was a Greek philosopher and polymath during the Classical period in Ancient Greece. Taught by Plato, he was the founder of the Peripatetic school of phil ...
(approx. 350 B.C.) reported illusory movement after viewing constant movement, but did not specify its direction. The first clear specification of the motion aftereffect was by
Jan Evangelista Purkyně Jan Evangelista Purkyně (; also written Johann Evangelist Purkinje) (17 or 18 December 1787 – 28 July 1869) was a Czech anatomist and physiologist. In 1839, he coined the term '' protoplasm'' for the fluid substance of a cell. He was one of t ...
(1820) who observed it after looking at a cavalry parade. Robert Addams (1834) reported the waterfall illusion after observing it at the
Falls of Foyers The Fall of Foyers (Scottish Gaelic: Eas na Smùide, meaning the smoking falls) is a waterfall on the River Foyers, which feeds Loch Ness, in Highland, Scotland, United Kingdom. It is located on the lower portion of the River Foyers, and drops . ...
in Scotland. According to Verstraten (1996) the term waterfall illusion was coined by Thompson (1880). According to Wade, Thompson, and Morgan, (2014), the most comprehensive single article on the phenomenon is by
Gustav Adolf Wohlgemuth Gustav Adolf Wohlgemuth (1 November 1868 – 2 March 1942), known as A. Wohlgemuth in his publications, was a German/British businessman who lived in England and made significant contributions to psychology, conducting research into visual percepti ...
(1911) https://archive.org/details/onaftereffectofs01wohl .


See also

*
Afterimage AfterImage is a Filipino rock band formed in 1986, best known for their songs "Habang May Buhay", "Next in Line", and "Mangarap Ka". They disbanded in 1997 and became active again in 2008 after they reunited and released their fourth studio album ...
* Motion perception


References

*Addams, R. (1834). An account of a peculiar optical phenomenon seen after having looked at a moving body. ''London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 5,'' 373–374 *Aristotle (approx. 350 B.C.) '' Parva Naturalia.'' *Barlow, H.B., & Hill, R.M. (1963). Evidence for a physiological explanation of the waterfall illusion. ''Nature, 200,'' 1345-1347. *Glasser, D. M., Tsui, J. M., Pack, C. C., & Tadin, D. (2011). Perceptual and neural consequences of rapid motion adaptation. ''PNAS Plus, 108''(45), E1080–E1088. *Petersen, S. E., Baker, J. F., & Allman, J. M. (1985). Direction-specific adaptation in area MT of the owl monkey, ''Brain Research, 346'', 146-150. *Purkinje, J. E. (1820) Beiträge zur näheren Kenntniss des Schwindels aus heautognostischen Daten. ''Medicinische Jahrbücher des kaiserlich-königlichen österreichischen Staates, 6,'' 79–125. *Srinivasan, M. V., & Dvorak, D. R. (1979). The waterfall illusion in an insect visual system. ''Vision Research, 19'', 1435-1437. *Thompson, P. (1880). Optical illusions of motion. ''Brain, 3,'' 289-298. *Tootell, R. B., Reppas, J. B., Dale, A. M., Look, R. B., Sereno, M. I., Malach, R., Brady, T. J., & Rosen, B. R. (1995), ''Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging, Nature, 375", 139-141. *Verstraten, F. A. J. (1996). On the ancient history of the direction of the motion aftereffect. ''Perception, 25,'' 1177-1188. *Wade, N. J., Thompson, P., & Morgan, M. (2014). The after-effect of Adolf Wohlgemuth’s seen motion. ''Perception, 43'', 229-234. doi: 10.1068/p4304ed *Wohlgemuth, A. (1911). On the after-effect of seen movement. ''British Journal of Psychology Monograph Supplement'', 1-117.


Bibliography

*Mather, G., Verstraten, F., & Anstis, S. (1998). ''The motion aftereffect: A modern perspective.'' Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press


External links

{{Commons category
"Strobe Illusion"
– an example of this effect (enable JavaScript to view).
"Spiral aftereffect"
– another example of this effect (enable JavaScript to view). Optical illusions Articles containing video clips