HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In
operant conditioning Operant conditioning, also called instrumental conditioning, is a learning process where behaviors are modified through the association of stimuli with reinforcement or punishment. In it, operants—behaviors that affect one's environment—are c ...
, the matching law is a quantitative relationship that holds between the relative rates of response and the relative
rates of reinforcement In behaviorism, rate of reinforcement is number of reinforcements per time, usually per minute. Symbol of this rate is usually ''Rf''. Its first major exponent was B.F. Skinner (1939). It is used in the Matching Law. ''Rf'' = ''# of reinforcemen ...
in
concurrent schedules of reinforcement In behavioral psychology, reinforcement is a consequence applied that will strengthen an organism's future behavior whenever that behavior is preceded by a specific antecedent stimulus. This strengthening effect may be measured as a higher freq ...
. For example, if two response alternatives A and B are offered to an organism, the ratio of response rates to A and B equals the ratio of reinforcements yielded by each response.Poling, A., Edwards, T. L., Weeden, M., & Foster, T. (2011). The matching law. ''Psychological Record'', 61(2), 313-322. This law applies fairly well when non-human subjects are exposed to concurrent variable interval schedules (but see below); its applicability in other situations is less clear, depending on the assumptions made and the details of the experimental situation. The generality of applicability of the matching law is subject of current debate. The matching law can be applied to situations involving a single response maintained by a single schedule of reinforcement if one assumes that alternative responses are always available to an organism, maintained by uncontrolled "extraneous" reinforcers. For example, an animal pressing a lever for food might pause for a drink of water. The matching law was first formulated by
R.J. Herrnstein Richard Julius Herrnstein (May 20, 1930 – September 13, 1994) was an American psychologist at Harvard University. He was an active researcher in animal learning in the Skinnerian tradition. Herrnstein was the Edgar Pierce Professor of Psychol ...
(1961) following an experiment with
pigeon Columbidae () is a bird family consisting of doves and pigeons. It is the only family in the order Columbiformes. These are stout-bodied birds with short necks and short slender bills that in some species feature fleshy ceres. They primarily ...
s on concurrent variable interval schedules. Pigeons were presented with two buttons in a
Skinner box An operant conditioning chamber (also known as a Skinner box) is a laboratory apparatus used to study animal behavior. The operant conditioning chamber was created by B. F. Skinner while he was a graduate student at Harvard University. The cham ...
, each of which led to varying rates of food reward. The pigeons tended to peck the button that yielded the greater food reward more often than the other button, and the ratio of their rates to the two buttons matched the ratio of their rates of reward on the two buttons.


Mathematical statement

If ''R'' and ''R'' are the
rate of response In behaviorism, rate of response is a ratio between two measurements with different units. Rate of responding is the number of responses per minute, or some other time unit. It is usually written as ''R''. Its first major exponent was B.F. Skinne ...
s on two schedules that yield obtained (as distinct from programmed) rates of reinforcement ''Rf'' and ''Rf'', the strict matching law holds that the relative response rate ''R'' / (''R'' + ''R'') ''matches'', that is, equals, the relative reinforcement rate ''Rf'' / (''Rf'' + ''Rf''). That is, :\frac=\frac This relationship can also be stated in terms of response and reinforcement ratios: :\frac=\frac Alternatively stated, it states that there exists a constant C for an individual animal, such that \frac=Cfor any i. That is, for an individual animal, the rate of response is proportional to rate of reinforcement for any task.


Deviations from matching, and the generalized matching law

A recent review by McDowell reveals that Herrnstein's original equation fails to accurately describe concurrent-schedule data under a substantial range of conditions. Three deviations from matching have been observed: undermatching, overmatching, and bias. Undermatching means that the response proportions are less extreme than the law predicts. Undermatching can happen if subjects too often switch between the two response options, a tendency that may be strengthened by reinforcers that happen to occur just after a subject switches. A changeover delay may be used to reduce the effectiveness of such post-switch reinforcers; typically, this is a 1.5 second interval after a switch when no reinforcer is presented. Overmatching is the opposite of undermatching, and is less common. Here the subjects response proportions are more extreme than reinforcement proportions. Overmatching may occur if there is a penalty for switching. A final deviation is bias, which occurs when subjects spend more time on one alternative than the matching equation predicts. This may happen if a subject prefers a certain environment, area in a laboratory, or method of responding. These failures of the matching law have led to the development of the "generalized matching law", which has parameters that reflect the deviations just described. The power law was first shown to fit operant choice data by Staddon (1968) and was generalized by Baum (1974). It has been found to fit a wide variety of matching data. The power law was shown by Mackay (1963) to be derivable from input and output logarithmic function and psychophysical and other behavioral data fitting this model were described by Staddon (1975). :\frac=b\left(\frac\right)^s This is more conveniently expressed in logarithmic form :\log\left(\frac\right)=\log\left(b\right)+s\cdot\log\left(\frac\right) The constants ''b'' and ''s'' are referred to as "bias" and "sensitivity" respectively. "Bias" reflects any tendency the subject may have to prefer one response over the other. "Sensitivity" reflects the degree to which the reinforcement ratio actually impacts the choice ratio. When this equation is plotted, the result is straight line; sensitivity changes the slope and bias changes the intercept of this line. The ''generalized matching law'' accounts for high proportions of the variance in most experiments on concurrent variable interval schedules in non-humans. Values of ''b'' often depend on details of the experiment set up, but values of ''s'' are consistently found to be around 0.8, whereas the value required for strict matching would be 1.0. The concurrent VI VI choice situation involves strong negative feedbacks: the longer the subject refrains from responding to an alternative, the higher his probability of payoff: switching is encouraged. Hinson and Staddon (1983) showed that power-law matching always results no matter what the balance between a transient tendency to "stay" vs. a growing tendency to switch after each reinforcement. Hence the molar law is compatible with many molecular choice processes.


Processes underlying the distribution of responses

There are three ideas on how humans and animals maximize reinforcement, molecular maximizing, molar maximizing and melioration. * molecular maximizing: organisms always choose whichever response alternative is most likely to be reinforced at the time. * molar maximizing: organisms distribute their responses among various alternatives so as to maximize the amount of reinforcement they earn over the long run. * melioration: literally means to "make better"; organisms respond so as to improve the local rates of reinforcement for response alternatives. behavior keeps shifting towards the better of two alternatives until ratios are equal-which makes matching.


Theoretical importance

The matching law is theoretically important for several reasons. First, it offers a simple quantification of behavior that can be applied to a number of situations. Secondly, offers a lawful account of choice. As Herrnstein (1970) expressed it, under an operant analysis, choice is nothing but behavior set into the context of other behavior. The matching law thus challenges the idea that choice is an unpredictable outcome of
free will Free will is the capacity of agents to choose between different possible courses of action unimpeded. Free will is closely linked to the concepts of moral responsibility, praise, culpability, sin, and other judgements which apply only to actio ...
, just as
B.F. Skinner Burrhus Frederic Skinner (March 20, 1904 – August 18, 1990) was an American psychologist, behaviorist, author, inventor, and social philosopher. He was a professor of psychology at Harvard University from 1958 until his retirement in 1974. ...
and others have argued. However this challenge becomes serious only if it applies to human behavior, as well as to the behavior of pigeons and other animals. When human participants perform under concurrent schedules of reinforcement, matching has been observed in some experiments, but wide deviations from matching have been found in others. Finally, if nothing else, the matching law is important because it has generated a great deal of research that has widened our understanding of operant control.


Relevance to psychopathology

The matching law, and the generalized matching law, have helped behavior analysts to understand some complex human behaviors, especially the behavior of children in certain conflict situations.James Snyder, Mike Stoolmiller, Gerald R. Patterson, Lynn Schrepferman, Jessica Oeser, Kassy Johnson, and Dana Soetaert (2003): The Application of Response Allocation Matching to Understanding Risk Mechanisms in Development: The Case of Young Children's Deviant Talk and Play, and Risk for Early-Onset Antisocial Behavior. ''The Behavior Analyst Today'', 4(4), 435–45 James Snyder and colleague have found that response matching predicts the use of conflict tactics by children and parents during conflict bouts.Snyder, J.J. & Patterson, G.R. (1995). Individualized differences in social aggression: A test of the reinforcement hypothesis in the natural environment. ''Behavior Therapy'', 26, 371–91. This matching rate predicts future arrests. Even children's use of deviant talk appears to follow a matching pattern.


Notes


References

* Baum, W.M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: Bias and undermatching. ''Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior'', 22, 231–42. * Bradshaw, C.M.; Szabadi, E. & Bevan, P. (1976). Behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of reinforcement ''Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior'', 26, 135–41. * Davison, M. & McCarthy, D. (1988). ''The matching law: A research review''. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. * Herrnstein, R.J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of responses as a function of frequency of reinforcement. ''Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour'', 4, 267–72. * Herrnstein, R.J. (1970). On the law of effect. ''Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior'', 13, 243–66. * Horne, P.J. & Lowe, C.F. (1993). Determinants of human performance on concurrent schedules. ''Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior'', 59, 29–60. {{doi, 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-29. * Poling, A., Edwards, T. L., Weeden, M., & Foster, T. (2011). The matching law. Psychological Record, 61(2), 313-322. * Simon, C., & Baum, W. M. (2017). Allocation of Speech in Conversation. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107. Behavioral concepts Behaviorism