![Lampropeltis triangulum annulata](https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/2/20/Lampropeltis_triangulum_annulata.jpg)
Emsleyan mimicry, also called Mertensian mimicry, describes an unusual type of
mimicry
In evolutionary biology, mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry f ...
where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species.
History
Emsleyan mimicry was first proposed by M. G. Emsley
as a possible explanation for how a predator species could learn to avoid an
aposematic phenotype of potentially dangerous animals, such as the
coral snake, when the predator is likely to die on its first encounter. The theory was developed by the German biologist
Wolfgang Wickler
Wolfgang Wickler is a German zoologist, behavioral researcher and author. He led the ethology, ethological department of the Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology from 1974, and he took over as director of the institute in 1975. Even aft ...
in a chapter of ''Mimicry in Plants and Animals'',
who named it after the
German herpetologist Robert Mertens
Robert Friedrich Wilhelm Mertens (1 December 1894 – 23 August 1975) was a German herpetologist. Several taxa of reptiles are named after him.Beolens, Bo; Watkins, Michael; Grayson, Michael (2011). ''The Eponym Dictionary of Reptiles''. Baltimore ...
.
Sheppard points out that Hecht and Marien had put forward a similar hypothesis ten years earlier.
Mimicry of a less deadly species
The scenario for Emsleyan mimicry is a little more difficult to understand than for other types of
mimicry
In evolutionary biology, mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry f ...
, since in other types of mimicry it is usually the most harmful species that is the model. But if a predator dies, it cannot
learn to recognize a warning signal, e.g., bright colours in a certain pattern. In other words, there is no advantage in being aposematic for an organism that is likely to kill any predator it succeeds in poisoning; such an
animal is better off being camouflaged, to avoid attacks altogether. If, however, there were some other species that were harmful but not deadly as well as aposematic, the predator could learn to recognize its particular warning colours and avoid such animals. A deadly species could then profit by mimicking the less dangerous aposematic organism if this reduces the number of attacks.
[
]
Non-Emsleyan mechanisms
Non-Emsleyan mechanisms that achieve the observed result, namely that predators avoid extremely deadly prey, are possible. Proposed alternatives include observational learning and innate avoidance. These provide alternative explanations to Emsleyan mimicry: if predators innately avoid a pattern then there is no need to suppose that the more deadly snake is mimicking the less deadly species in these cases.
Observational learning
One mechanism is observational learning
Observational learning is learning that occurs through observing the behavior of others. It is a form of social learning which takes various forms, based on various processes. In humans, this form of learning seems to not need reinforcement to oc ...
, for example through watching a conspecific die. The observing predator then remembers that the prey is deadly and avoids it. Jouventin and colleagues conducted exploratory tests on baboons in 1977 that suggested this was possible.
Innate avoidance
Another possible mechanism is that a predator might not have to learn that a certain prey is harmful in the first place: it could have instinct
Instinct is the inherent inclination of a living organism towards a particular complex behaviour, containing both innate (inborn) and learned elements. The simplest example of an instinctive behaviour is a fixed action pattern (FAP), in which a v ...
ive genetic programming to avoid certain signals. In this case, other organisms could benefit from this programming, and Batesian
Batesian mimicry is a form of mimicry where a harmless species has evolved to imitate the warning signals of a harmful species directed at a predator of them both. It is named after the English naturalist Henry Walter Bates, after his work on Bu ...
or Müllerian mimics of it could potentially evolve.
Some species indeed do innately recognize certain aposematic patterns. Hand-reared turquoise-browed motmots (''Eumomota superciliosa''), avian predators, instinctively avoid snakes with red and yellow rings. Other colours with the same pattern, and even red and yellow ''stripes'' with the same width as rings, were tolerated. However, models with red and yellow rings were feared, with the birds flying away and giving alarm calls in some cases.
The models would not have to be other snakes. Large red and black millipedes are common and foul-tasting; various species of these millipedes form Müllerian mimicry rings, and some are the models for mimicry in lizards.
Coral snake system
Some harmless milk snake (''Lampropeltis triangulum'') subspecies, the moderately toxic false coral snakes
False coral may refer to many species of snakes, including:
*several species of the genus ''Erythrolamprus'':
** ''Erythrolamprus aesculapii'', a mildly venomous colubrid snake species found in South America.
** ''Erythrolamprus bizona'', a harmle ...
(genus ''Erythrolamprus''), and the deadly coral snakes (genus '' Micrurus'') all have a red background color with black and either white or yellow rings. Over 115 species or some 18% of snakes in the New World are within this mimicry system. In this system, Emsley stated that both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are the mimics, whereas the false coral snakes are the models.[
It has been suggested that this system could be an instance of pseudomimicry, the similar colour patterns having evolved independently in similar habitats.]
References
{{Mimicry
Mimicry