HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Lexicalist hypothesis is a hypothesis proposed by
Noam Chomsky Avram Noam Chomsky (born December 7, 1928) is an American public intellectual: a linguist, philosopher, cognitive scientist, historian, social critic, and political activist. Sometimes called "the father of modern linguistics", Chomsky is ...
in which he claims that syntactic transformations only can operate on syntactic constituents. The hypothesis states that the system of grammar that assembles words is separate and different from the system of grammar that assembles phrases out of words. There are two versions of lexicalist hypothesis: "weak" and "strong." In the "weak" version the transformations could not operate on the derivational words; whereas in "strong" approach, the transformations can neither operate on derivational and inflectional words. Lexicalist hypothesis is a response to generative semanticians who use transformations in the derivation of complex words. There are objections to the hypothesis such as
distributed morphology In generative linguistics, Distributed Morphology is a theoretical framework introduced in 1993 by Morris Halle and Alec Marantz.Halle, Morris & Alec Marantz. 1993. 'Distributed Morphology and the Pieces of Inflection.' In The View from Buildin ...
. The
Lexical Integrity Hypothesis The Lexical Integrity Hypothesis (LIH) or Lexical Integrity Principle is a hypothesis in linguistics which states that syntactic transformations do not apply to subparts of words. It functions as a constraint on transformational grammar. Words a ...
is a subset of the Lexicalist Hypothesis.


History

In the 1950s,
Noam Chomsky Avram Noam Chomsky (born December 7, 1928) is an American public intellectual: a linguist, philosopher, cognitive scientist, historian, social critic, and political activist. Sometimes called "the father of modern linguistics", Chomsky is ...
introduced generative grammar into the world of linguistics and his theory quickly became widely accepted and popular. He mentioned multiple theories of grammar but he favoured the theory to which he gave a name of
transformational generative grammar In linguistics, transformational grammar (TG) or transformational-generative grammar (TGG) is part of the theory of generative grammar, especially of natural languages. It considers grammar to be a system of rules that generate exactly those combin ...
in particular.
Transformational generative grammar In linguistics, transformational grammar (TG) or transformational-generative grammar (TGG) is part of the theory of generative grammar, especially of natural languages. It considers grammar to be a system of rules that generate exactly those combin ...
states that sentences are formed using the standard
generative grammar Generative grammar, or generativism , is a linguistic theory that regards linguistics as the study of a hypothesised innate grammatical structure. It is a biological or biologistic modification of earlier structuralist theories of linguistic ...
rules to form what is called a deep structure and from there, rules called transformations are used to change this structure by adding, moving, changing or even deleting material from the present structure. In 1960, Robert B. Lees brought up a theory that derived nominals are all
deverbal Deverbal nouns are nouns that are derived from verbs or verb phrases. The formation of deverbal nouns is a type of nominalization (noun formation). Examples of deverbal nouns in English include ''organization'' (derived from the verb ''organize'') ...
and not sentential. In other words, the theory states that nominalizations are not derived from verbs or sentences. Lees' theory prevailed until
Noam Chomsky Avram Noam Chomsky (born December 7, 1928) is an American public intellectual: a linguist, philosopher, cognitive scientist, historian, social critic, and political activist. Sometimes called "the father of modern linguistics", Chomsky is ...
released ''Remarks on Nominalization'' in 1970. It was in ''Remarks on Nominalization'', that
Chomsky Avram Noam Chomsky (born December 7, 1928) is an American public intellectual: a linguist, philosopher, cognitive scientist, historian, social critic, and political activist. Sometimes called "the father of modern linguistics", Chomsky is ...
proposed the lexicalist hypothesis. Both Lees' and
Chomsky Avram Noam Chomsky (born December 7, 1928) is an American public intellectual: a linguist, philosopher, cognitive scientist, historian, social critic, and political activist. Sometimes called "the father of modern linguistics", Chomsky is ...
's theories are in support of
generative grammar Generative grammar, or generativism , is a linguistic theory that regards linguistics as the study of a hypothesised innate grammatical structure. It is a biological or biologistic modification of earlier structuralist theories of linguistic ...
.


Theory

The lexicalist hypothesis can be dissected into the following aspects.


Idiosyncrasy argument

The relationship between derived nominals and the corresponding verb from which it is derived, is idiosyncratic and highly irregular. This means morphological transformations of the verb into its derived nominals have no regulations. For example, there is no apparent morphology-to-meaning link between the verb ''profess'' (meaning 'to claim or declare openly'), and its derived nominals such as ''professor'' (meaning ‘university teacher’) or ''profession'' (meaning 'career'). In this example, changing verbs to their derived nominals involves the addition of '''-or' and '-ion, which do not inherently add a new dimension of meaning to the verb, indicating that these morphological additions are idiosyncratic, and not systematic. Another example would be the verb ''ignore'' (refuse to acknowledge), and its derived nominals ''ignorance'' (lack of knowledge) and ''ignoramus'' (a stupid person). The morphological transformations made to the verb ''ignore'' are largely idiosyncratic, and do not inherently contribute to the meaning.


Internal structure argument

Structures in which derived nominals occur resemble that of usual
noun phrase In linguistics, a noun phrase, or nominal (phrase), is a phrase that has a noun or pronoun as its head or performs the same grammatical function as a noun. Noun phrases are very common cross-linguistically, and they may be the most frequently oc ...
s, rather than following the structures of the
verb phrase In linguistics, a verb phrase (VP) is a syntactic unit composed of a verb and its arguments except the subject of an independent clause or coordinate clause. Thus, in the sentence ''A fat man quickly put the money into the box'', the words ''quic ...
that the original verb sits in. Using the verb ''refuse'' and its derived nominal ''refusal'' for instance: * We refuse the offer. ''(Verb phrase)'' * *We refusal the offer. ''(Replacing the verb with its corresponding derived nominal in the same verb phrase structure)'' n asterisk (*) in linguistics denotes an ungrammatical form. The derived nominal does not occur in the structure of the corresponding verb in which it is derived from. Instead, it should occur in the structure of a generic noun phrase, which can contain
determiner A determiner, also called determinative (abbreviated ), is a word, phrase, or affix that occurs together with a noun or noun phrase and generally serves to express the reference of that noun or noun phrase in the context. That is, a determiner m ...
s,
adjective In linguistics, an adjective (list of glossing abbreviations, abbreviated ) is a word that generally grammatical modifier, modifies a noun or noun phrase or describes its referent. Its semantic role is to change information given by the noun. Tra ...
s, and
prepositional phrases An adpositional phrase, in linguistics, is a syntactic category that includes ''prepositional phrases'', ''postpositional phrases'', and ''circumpositional phrases''. Adpositional phrases contain an adposition (preposition, postposition, or circ ...
: * The unrelenting quest of the three musketeers. ''(Noun phrase)'' * The stupid refusal of the offer. ''(Using a derived nominal in a structure similar to a generic noun phrase)''


Frozen structure argument

Derived nominals correspond to base structures, that is, to treat derived nominals as deep structure nouns. According to Chomsky, (a) and (b) are preferable to (c) and (d). (a) His looking up of the information. (b) His defining away of the problem. (c) *His looking of the information up. (d) *His defining of the problem away. With this example, Chomsky demonstrates that particle shift, a process in which the phrase is split up and shifted to another part of the sentence, is not applicable for derived nominal phrases. Other processes that cannot be applied to derived nominals include: *
Dative shift In linguistics, dative shift refers to a pattern in which the subcategorization of a verb can take on two alternating forms, the oblique dative form or the double object construction form. In the oblique dative (OD) form, the verb takes a noun ph ...
* There-insertion: Inserting ''"there"'' into a derived nominal phrase. * Transforming a
raising Raising may refer to: *Raising (linguistics), a syntactic construction *Raising (phonetics), a sound change *Raising (metalworking), a metalworking technique *Barn raising, a community event to erect the wooden framework for a building *Fundraising ...
to an
object Object may refer to: General meanings * Object (philosophy), a thing, being, or concept ** Object (abstract), an object which does not exist at any particular time or place ** Physical object, an identifiable collection of matter * Goal, an ai ...
* Psych-Movement: Moving the object of the psych-verb (a verb that expresses a mental state or event, also called a mental verb) into the subject position, while the original subject is made into a prepositional phrase. * Tough-Movement Originally, these processes could not be applied to base nouns, and therefore, according to the frozen structure argument, they cannot be applied to derived nominals as well.


Criticisms

Many theorists have come up with examples that seem to undermine the strength of lexicalist hypothesis. Advocates of the lexicalist hypothesis posit certain properties of syntactic words as evidence for a pre-syntactic word-formation module. Michael Barrie (2012) of Sogang University discusses these properties and argues that none of them presents a solid case for positing a word-building module distinct from syntax. For example, a syntactic analysis of word formation predicts that the components of a syntactically formed word should have the same syntactic properties as when it appears as an independent word, which can be counted as the property of inheritance. In this regard, Ackema & Neeleman (2004) address the derivation of the word ''driver''. If the verb ''driver'' is syntactically constructed from the verb ''drive'' and the
nominalizer In linguistics, nominalization or nominalisation is the use of a word that is not a noun (e.g., a verb, an adjective or an adverb) as a noun, or as the head of a noun phrase. This change in functional category can occur through morphological tra ...
''-er'', the verb should maintain its verbal properties and be able to take an object, resulting in the ungrammatical ''*driver a truck''. Then Ackema & Neeleman (2004) come up with various structures that defend this property. Barrie (2012) examines these structures and concludes that their argument in terms of inheritance does not present a valid argument in support of an independent, pre-syntactic word-forming module. Bruening (2018) argues that lexicalist hypothesis, mainly the part that concerns
lexical integrity hypothesis The Lexical Integrity Hypothesis (LIH) or Lexical Integrity Principle is a hypothesis in linguistics which states that syntactic transformations do not apply to subparts of words. It functions as a constraint on transformational grammar. Words a ...
, which assumes that words are separate units that are inserted into syntactic structures, is fundamentally incorrect. He points out three ways in which he believes that the lexicalist hypothesis is mistaken in its view of grammar.


Error 1: Phrasal Syntax Can Feed Word Formation

According to the lexicalist hypothesis, interaction between the word and phrase systems is unidirectional: the output of the
word-formation In linguistics, word formation is an ambiguous term that can refer to either: * the processes through which words can change (i.e. morphology), or * the creation of new lexemes in a particular language Morphological A common method of word form ...
system provides the input to the phrasal syntax, and not vice versa. (1) a. She had that ''don’t-you-dare!'' look. b. She had that ''I’m-so-proud-of-myself'' look. (1) shows that the phrases that constitute the first member of these compounds MUST be put together by the
syntax In linguistics, syntax () is the study of how words and morphemes combine to form larger units such as phrases and sentences. Central concerns of syntax include word order, grammatical relations, hierarchical sentence structure ( constituency) ...
, because they have the form that the
syntax In linguistics, syntax () is the study of how words and morphemes combine to form larger units such as phrases and sentences. Central concerns of syntax include word order, grammatical relations, hierarchical sentence structure ( constituency) ...
requires. As in 1a, they can have the form of a declarative. However, if the first member of the compound violates rules and constraints of the phrasal syntax, the result is ill-formed, as in (2). (2) a. *She had that ''you-don’t-dare!'' look.     (obligatory inversion with negative imperative) b. *She had that ''myself-is-so-proud-of-me'' look.    (reflexive bad as subject) If these phrases were not put together by the phrasal syntax but by some other mechanism, that mechanism would have to precisely duplicate the constraints of the phrasal syntax.


Error 2: Phrasal Syntax Has Access To Subword Units

Bruening (2018) also argues that the lexicalist hypothesis is incorrect in its assertion that the phrasal syntax has no access to subword units. (3) a. You can pre- or re-mix it. b. *They produce cranber- and dai-ry products. (4) a. bi- and a-sexual b. birth- and adopted sons c. *bi- and ma-son paraphernalia (bison paraphernalia and mason paraphernalia) Bruening analyzes that in (4), for example, that the strings ''bi-'' and ''a-'', as well as the string ''son'', can stand alone. However, (4c) does not function because the morphemes ''bi-'', ''ma-'' (which sounds identical to ''a-''), and son are not distinct in these terms. This means that ellipsis can't only work with phonological or prosodic strings; it also requires access to morphological structure.


Error 3: Morphology And Syntax Obey The Same Principles

According to the lexicalist hypothesis, as distinct components of grammar, the
word-formation In linguistics, word formation is an ambiguous term that can refer to either: * the processes through which words can change (i.e. morphology), or * the creation of new lexemes in a particular language Morphological A common method of word form ...
system and the phrasal system can be assumed to follow different principles. Bruening looks at some of the claimed distinctions between word formation and phrasal syntax, and concludes that none of those distinctions are real, and therefore
morphology Morphology, from the Greek and meaning "study of shape", may refer to: Disciplines * Morphology (archaeology), study of the shapes or forms of artifacts * Morphology (astronomy), study of the shape of astronomical objects such as nebulae, galaxies ...
and
syntax In linguistics, syntax () is the study of how words and morphemes combine to form larger units such as phrases and sentences. Central concerns of syntax include word order, grammatical relations, hierarchical sentence structure ( constituency) ...
follow the same rules.


Notes

{{reflist


References

* Ackema, P., & Neeleman, A. (2004-10-07). Beyond Morphology: Interface Conditions on Word Formation. : Oxford University Press. Retrieved 1 Apr. 2021, from https://oxford.universitypressscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199267286.001.0001/acprof-9780199267286. *Barrie, M. (2012). Noun incorporation and the lexicalist hypothesis. ''Studies in Generative Grammar'', ''22''(2), 235-261. *Bruening, B. (2018). The lexicalist hypothesis: Both wrong and superfluous. Language 94(1), 1-42. doi:10.1353/lan.2018.0000. *Chomsky, N. 1970. Remarks on Nominalization, in: Jacobs, R. and P. Rosenbaum (eds.) Readings in English Transformational Grammar, Blaisdell, Waltham, MA. * Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In H. K, & S. J. Keyser, The View from Building 20. Essays in Linguistics in Honour of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press. Generative syntax Linguistic morphology Syntax