Kashaya Language
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Kashaya (also Southwestern Pomo, Kashia) is the critically endangered
language Language is a structured system of communication. The structure of a language is its grammar and the free components are its vocabulary. Languages are the primary means by which humans communicate, and may be conveyed through a variety of ...
of the Kashia band of the
Pomo The Pomo are an Indigenous people of California. Historical Pomo territory in Northern California was large, bordered by the Pacific Coast to the west, extending inland to Clear Lake, and mainly between Cleone and Duncans Point. One small ...
people. The
Pomoan languages The Pomoan, or Pomo , languages are a small family of seven languages indigenous to northern California spoken by the Pomo people, whose ancestors lived in the valley of the Russian River and the Clear Lake basin. Four languages are extinct, an ...
have been classified as part of the
Hokan language family The Hokan language family is a hypothetical grouping of a dozen small language families that were spoken mainly in California, Arizona and Baja California. Etymology The name ''Hokan'' is loosely based on the word for "two" in the various Hokan ...
(although the status of Hokan itself is controversial). The name ''Kashaya'' corresponds to words in neighboring languages with meanings such as "skillful" and "expert gambler". It is spoken by the
Kashia Band of Pomo Indians of the Stewarts Point Rancheria The Kashia Band of Pomo Indians of the Stewarts Point Rancheria is a federally recognized tribe of Pomo people in Sonoma County, California.Pritzker, Barry M. ''A Native American Encyclopedia: History, Culture, and Peoples''. Oxford: Oxford Univ ...
.


Phonology


Vowels

Kashaya has five vowels, which all occur as short and long. In the orthography established by Robert Oswalt, long vowels are represented by a raised dot (ꞏ). Vowel length is contrastive in pairs such as "bone" versus "wind", and "hill, mountain" versus "uphill".


Consonants

Kashaya has the consonants shown in the chart below, following the transcription style established by Oswalt (1961). The letter c represents the affricate , which patterns phonologically as a palatal stop. The coronal stops differ not so much in the location of the contact against the top of the mouth as in the configuration of the tongue. The dental stop t is described by Oswalt (1961) as post-dental among older speakers but as interdental among younger speakers more heavily influenced by English, similar to the place of articulation of . This dental stop has a
laminal A laminal consonant is a phone (speech sound) produced by obstructing the air passage with the blade of the tongue, the flat top front surface just behind the tip of the tongue in contact with upper lip, teeth, alveolar ridge, to possibly, as ...
articulation perhaps best transcribed in IPA as . The alveolar stop ṭ is an
apical Apical means "pertaining to an apex". It may refer to: *Apical ancestor, refers to the last common ancestor of an entire group, such as a species (biology) or a clan (anthropology) *Apical (anatomy), an anatomical term of location for features loc ...
articulation, more precisely . For younger speakers it resembles the English t in position. This chart treats aspirated and glottalized sonorants as single segments; Oswalt analyzes them as sequences of a sonorant plus or , from which they often derive. The consonants occur only in loanwords; due to the influence of English, loans from Spanish and Russian receive a pronunciation of like that in American English. The voiced stops are the realization of in onset position.


Syllable structure

In the normal case, every syllable requires a single
onset Onset may refer to: *Onset (audio), the beginning of a musical note or sound *Onset, Massachusetts Onset is a census-designated place (CDP) in the town of Wareham, Massachusetts, United States. The population was 1,573 at the 2010 census. Geog ...
consonant; no onset clusters are permitted. In most contexts, the rhyme consists of a vowel that may be long or followed by a single consonant in the coda, resulting in the possible syllables CV, CV꞉, and CVC. Examples of these structures are "coyote", "is running (non-final)", and "to bow". A few
loanword A loanword (also loan word or loan-word) is a word at least partly assimilated from one language (the donor language) into another language. This is in contrast to cognates, which are words in two or more languages that are similar because t ...
s do have an onset cluster, such as "bridle" and "stove" (from
Spanish Spanish might refer to: * Items from or related to Spain: **Spaniards are a nation and ethnic group indigenous to Spain **Spanish language, spoken in Spain and many Latin American countries **Spanish cuisine Other places * Spanish, Ontario, Can ...
, ). Loans may also have superheavy CV꞉C syllables, since stressed vowels in the source language are typically borrowed with a long vowel: "match", "fault", "brick" (Spanish , ;
Russian Russian(s) refers to anything related to Russia, including: *Russians (, ''russkiye''), an ethnic group of the East Slavic peoples, primarily living in Russia and neighboring countries *Rossiyane (), Russian language term for all citizens and peo ...
"oven"). An exceptional word with CVCC is "gnat". Superheavy CV꞉C and CVCC syllables are well attested word-finally in specific verb forms. For example, the Suppositional suffix can be final as in yielding "he must have run down". More typically a superheavy syllable occurs when the rightmost suffix is one of several evidential suffixes containing an vowel that deletes when no other suffix follows, such as the Circumstantial in "he must have drowned" and the Visual in "I saw it run in".


Stress

The determination of
stress Stress may refer to: Science and medicine * Stress (biology), an organism's response to a stressor such as an environmental condition * Stress (linguistics), relative emphasis or prominence given to a syllable in a word, or to a word in a phrase ...
is quite complex and the main stress can fall on any of the first five syllables in a phrase, depending on various factors. According to the analysis in Buckley (1994), iambs are constructed from left to right and the leftmost foot generally receives the main stress: "I ran in", "he is peeking in there". Non-initial feet do not receive secondary stress but lead to lengthening of vowels in open syllables (which however does not apply to word-final vowels nor to a large set of suffixes occurring toward the end of the word). The initial syllable is extrametrical unless the word begins with a
monosyllabic In linguistics, a monosyllable is a word or utterance of only one syllable. It is most commonly studied in the fields of phonology and morphology and it has no semantic content. The word has originated from the Greek language. "Yes", "no", "jump", ...
root, as in the case of "run". For example, the footing in "start to cut downward" with the root "cut" skips the first syllable, while in "keep running all the way around" this is blocked by the short root "run". The pattern is further complicated when the first foot begins on a syllable that has a long vowel, as in "tell". If the following syllable is closed, the stress shifts to the foot that contains that syllable: "cause to bring a message out here". If the long vowel is followed by a CV syllable, i.e. if the initial sequence to be footed is CV꞉CV, the length moves rightward to create CVCV꞉ and the stress similarly shifts to the next foot: "bring a message out!". Combined with extrametricality, this can lead to stress as far in as the fifth syllable: "always be too shy" from the root "be shy"; this verb forms a minimal pair with "gather", which lacks stress shift in "always gather". While iambic lengthening is determined by footing within a word, stress can be reassigned at the phrasal level across word boundaries: "be good!" where is the adjective "good" and the remainder is the imperative verb.


Phonological processes

A large number of processes affect the realization of underlying sounds in Kashaya. A representative sample is given here. * The glottalized nasals surface unchanged in the syllable coda, but change to voiced stops in the onset: cf. the root "see, look" in "if he sees" and "look!". * The default vowel changes to after , and to after (from underlying ): cf. the imperative in "limp!", "punch him!", "look!". * Any vowel changes to /a/ after a uvular: /ʔusaq-in/ → "while washing the face", /sima꞉q-eti/ → "although he's asleep". * Plain stops are aspirated in the coda: /da-hyut-meʔ/ → "break it!" (formal imperative); cf. /da-hyut-i/ → "break it!" (informal). * A uvular stop in the coda generally loses its place of articulation: /sima꞉q-ti/ → "about to fall asleep". Exceptions exist before certain suffixes and in loanwords such as "dress" (from
Alutiiq The Alutiiq people (pronounced in English; from Promyshlenniki Russian Алеутъ, "Aleut"; plural often "Alutiit"), also called by their ancestral name ( or ; plural often "Sugpiat"), as well as Pacific Eskimo or Pacific Yupik, are a so ...
).
Debuccalization Debuccalization or deoralization is a sound change or alternation in which an oral consonant loses its original place of articulation and moves it to the glottis (usually , , or ). The pronunciation of a consonant as is sometimes called aspir ...
of other stops occurs in various contexts as well. * An aspirated stop in a prefix dissimilates from an /h/ or an aspirated stop at the beginning of the root, similar to Grassmann's Law: cf. the prefix /pʰu/ "by blowing" with aspiration in "be blown along" but without it in "a windbreak".


Morphology

Kashaya can be classified as a
polysynthetic language In linguistic typology, polysynthetic languages, formerly holophrastic languages, are highly synthetic languages, i.e. languages in which words are composed of many morphemes (word parts that have independent meaning but may or may not be able ...
; it is primarily suffixing but has an important set of instrumental prefixes on verbs.


Nouns

Noun morphology is modest. The main examples are prefixes that mark possession of kinship terms. The first person has several allomorphs including the prefix and CV꞉ reduplication; the latter is informal and is associated with phonologically less marked stems, no doubt derived historically from child pronunciations. The prefixes , , mark second, third, and reflexive ("one's own"). These prefixes occur with the suffixes , depending on the stem and prefix. Examples with /qa/ "grandmother" are "your ~", "his/her/their ~", and informal "my grandma", based on /ka/ simplified from /qa/.


Verbs

Verbs take a great variety of suffixes divided into many position classes. There are also instrumental prefixes that figure crucially in the use of many verb stems.


Position classes

Oswalt (1961) identifies the following position classes; it can be seen that there is far more complexity in the set of suffixes than in the prefixes. * Prefixes ** A — Instrumental ** B — Plural Act * Root * Inner Group Suffixes ** I — Plural Agent ** II — Reduplication ** III — Essive, Terrestrial ** IV — Semelfactive, Inceptive, Plural Act, Plural Movement * Middle Group Suffixes ** Va — Directionals ** Vb — Directionals/Inceptives ** VI — Reflexive, Reciprocal ** VII — Causative ** VIII — Locomotory ** IX — Durative ** X — Distributive * Outer Group Suffixes ** XI — Defunctive ** XII — Negative ** XIII — First Person Object, Remote Past, Inferential ** XIV — Evidentials, Modals, Imperatives, Futures, Absolutive, Adverbializers ** XVv — Nonfinal Verb, Responsive, Interrogative ** XVn — Subjective, Objective ** XVb — Explanatory Only a few of the most important categories can be illustrated here.


Instrumental prefixes

Many verbs cannot occur without a prefix that provides information about the manner of the action described. These 20 instrumental prefixes, all of the shape CV, are the following. * ''ba-'' "with the lips, snout, or beak; by speech (or hearing)" * ''bi-'' "by encircling, e.g. with the arms; by sewing, eating (esp. with a spoon)" * ''ca-'' "with the rear end, a massive or bulky object, a knife" * ''cu-'' "with a round object, flowing water, the front end; by shooting" * ''cʰi-'' "by holding a small part of a larger object, e.g. a handle" * ''da-'' "with the hand (palm), paw; by waves" * ''du-'' "with the finger" * ''di-'' "by gravity, falling, a heavy weight" * ''ha-'' "with a swinging motion" * ''hi-'' "with the body" * ''ma-'' "with the sole of the foot, claws, the butt of the hand" * ''mi-'' "with the small end of a long object, the toes, nose; by kicking, smelling, counting, reading" * ''mu-'' "with a quick movement, heat, light, mind or emotions" * ''pʰa-'' "with the end of a long object, the fist; by wrapping" * ''pʰi-'' "with the side of a long object, the eyes, an ax, a hammer" * ''pʰu-'' "by blowing" * ''qa-'' "between forces: with the teeth, by chewing, eating" * ''si-'' "by water: wetting, dissolving, slipping, floating, rain, tongue" * ''ša-'' "by a long object moving lengthwise; with a mesh" * ''šu-'' "by pulling, pushing and pulling; with a long flexible object" For example, the root /hcʰa/ "knock over" can occur unprefixed as "fall over" where no agency is indicated, but is typically prefixed to expand upon the meaning: "knock over with snout", "throw someone in wrestling", "knock over by backing into", "push over with the hand", "push over with the finger", "be knocked over by a falling object", etc.


Suffixes

A sampling of verb suffixes: * Directionals include ''-ad'' "along, here", ''-mul'' "around", ''-mad'' "in an enclosed or defined place", ''-aq'' "out from here; north or west from here". * Directionals/Inceptives ''-ala'' "down" and ''-ibic'' "up, away" also mark the beginning of an action. * Causative ''-hqa''. * Durative ''-ad'' with many other allomorphs, such as ''-id'', ''-cid'', ''-med'', depending on the preceding segment and the length of the stem. * Evidentials include quotative ''-do'', circumstantial ''-qa'', and visual ''-ya''. The /a/ of the evidentials deletes when no other suffix follows. * Absolutive ''-w'' after vowels, ''-u'' after /d/, and ''-ʔ'' after other consonants. Position class XIV (Evidentials, Modals, Imperatives, Futures, Absolutive, Adverbializers) represents the largest set of suffixes and is the only slot that is obligatorily filled in every verb. A few examples of verbs with many affixes, the root shown in bold: * ''pʰa-ʔdi-c-á꞉d-ala-w'' "to poke with the end of a stick while moving downhill" * ''cʰi-ʔdí-ccicʼ-a꞉dad-u'' "to walk along picking up things and pulling them close to oneself" * ''nohpʰo-yíʔ-ciʔ-do'' "it's said that those former people used to live (like that)"


Syntax

The basic word order of Kashaya is quite flexible in main clauses; however, the default location for the verb is final, and this position is required in subordinate clauses. A notable feature is that when a verb does occur in non-final position, depending on other suffixes present it takes the Nonfinal Verb ending ''-e꞉''. Some possible orders are illustrated here with the simple sentence "I see that dog", containing the elements ''ʔa'' "I (subj)", ''mul'' "that (obj)", ''hayu'' "dog", ''canʼ-'' "see". * ''hayu mul ʔa canʼ'' * ''hayu ʔa mul canʼ'' * ''hayu ʔa cade꞉ mul'' * ''cade꞉ ʔa hayu mul'' Oswalt (1961) reports that younger speakers tend to favor the SVO order typical of English.


Case marking

The most important case markers are subjective and objective case. (Others are the vocative and comitative, of more limited application.) Most nouns are marked with the subjective ''ʔem'' or the objective ''ʔel''; these are morphologically complex and contain the actual case markers /m/ and /l/, found with verbal expressions. * ''ʔacacʼ em ʔima꞉ta ʔél cadu'' — "the man (''ʔacac'') sees the woman (''ʔima꞉ta'')" * ''ʔahca qáwiwa-l cadé꞉ ʔa'' — "I see the house (''ʔahca'') he is building (''qawiwa-'')" Personal names take the suffix ''-to'' in the objective case, zero in the subjective. Pronouns have distinct forms in subjective and objective case; the forms are not easily analyzed but the objective case generally ends in ''-(a)l'' or ''-to''. Demonstratives are also distinguished for case; they are given here as subjective/objective: * ''mu(꞉) / mul'' — "that, this, it, those, these, they (vague demonstrative or anaphoric reference)" * ''maʔu / maʔal'' — "this, these (the closer object)" * ''haʔu / haʔal'' — "that, those (the further object)"


Switch reference

Switch reference In linguistics, switch-reference (SR) describes any clause-level morpheme that signals whether certain prominent arguments in 'adjacent' clauses are coreferential. In most cases, it marks whether the subject of the verb in one clause is corefe ...
refers to markings according to whether a subordinate verb has the same or different subject as the main verb. In Kashaya it also marks whether the time of the action is the same, or preceding the main verb action in the past or future. There is no consistent expression of these categories except for the element /pʰi/ in both future suffixes, but the remaining /la/ is not identifiable as a separate suffix. The suffix containing /li/ is realized as ''-wli'' after vowels, ''-u꞉li'' (or /uwli/) after ''d'', and ''-ʔli'' after other consonants; this allomorphy is related to that of the very common Absolutive suffix, ''-w, -u, -ʔ''. A few examples of these morphemes: * ''tʼeti꞉bícʰ-pʰi maya miyícʼkʰe'' — "you should stand up and (then) speak" ame subject, future tense* ''pʰala cóhtoʔ, duwecí꞉d-em'' — "he left again as night was falling" ifferent subject, simultaneous* ''cohtóʔ da꞉qacʼ-ba cohtó꞉y'' — "having wanting to go, he went" ame subject, past tense* ''ʔama: qʰaʔa꞉dú-ʔli, cohtoʔ'' — "after morning had come, she left" ifferent subject, past tense consonant-final stem /qʰaʔa-aduc/


Notable Kashaya Pomo speakers

* Pomo speaker Langford "Lanny" Roger Pinola (April 25, 1938 – April 21, 2003) lived on the Kashaya Reservation until age six. *
Essie Pinola Parrish Essie Pinola Parrish (1902–1979), was a Kashaya Pomo spiritual leader and exponent of native traditions. She was also a notable basket weaver. Biography Parrish was born Essie Pinola in 1902 at the Stewarts Point Rancheria in Stewarts Poin ...
(1902–1979), a noted basketweaver, educated Kashaya children in the language, and "compiled a Kashaya Pomo dictionary, working with Robert Oswalt, a Berkeley scholar well-known in the field of Indian linguistics."


See also

*
Pomoan languages The Pomoan, or Pomo , languages are a small family of seven languages indigenous to northern California spoken by the Pomo people, whose ancestors lived in the valley of the Russian River and the Clear Lake basin. Four languages are extinct, an ...
*
Pomo people The Pomo are an Indigenous people of California. Historical Pomo territory in Northern California was large, bordered by the Pacific Coast to the west, extending inland to Clear Lake, and mainly between Cleone and Duncans Point. One small gr ...
*
Fort Ross Fort Ross ( Russian: Форт-Росс, Kashaya ''mé·ṭiʔni''), originally Fortress Ross ( pre-reformed Russian: Крѣпость Россъ, tr. ''Krepostʹ Ross''), is a former Russian establishment on the west coast of North America i ...


Notes


References

* Buckley, Eugene (1994). Theoretical aspects of Kashaya phonology and morphology. CSLI Publications, Stanford University. * * McLendon, Sally. (2003). Evidentials in Eastern Pomo with a comparative survey of the category in other Pomoan languages. In A. Y. Aikhenvald & R. M. W. Dixon (Eds.), ''Studies in evidentiality''(pp. 101–129). Typological studies in language (Vol. 54). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. ; . * Mithun, Marianne. (1999). ''The languages of Native North America''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (hbk); . * Oswalt, Robert L. (1961). A Kashaya grammar (Southwestern Pomo), PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.


External links


Collection of Kashaya resources
compiled by the University of Pennsylvania
Kashaya dictionaryKashaya language
overview at the Survey of California and Other Indian Languages
Kashaya basic lexicon at the Global Lexicostatistical Database

OLAC resources in and about the Kashaya language
{{DEFAULTSORT:Kashaya Language Pomo tribe Indigenous languages of California Endangered Pomoan languages Pomoan languages History of Sonoma County, California