Grammatical Theory
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In
linguistics Linguistics is the scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure. Linguis ...
, syntax () is the study of how words and
morpheme A morpheme is the smallest meaningful Constituent (linguistics), constituent of a linguistic expression. The field of linguistics, linguistic study dedicated to morphemes is called morphology (linguistics), morphology. In English, morphemes are ...
s combine to form larger units such as phrases and sentences. Central concerns of syntax include
word order In linguistics, word order (also known as linear order) is the order of the syntactic constituents of a language. Word order typology studies it from a cross-linguistic perspective, and examines how different languages employ different orders. C ...
,
grammatical relations In linguistics, grammatical relations (also called grammatical functions, grammatical roles, or syntactic functions) are functional relationships between constituents in a clause. The standard examples of grammatical functions from traditional gra ...
, hierarchical sentence structure (
constituency An electoral district, also known as an election district, legislative district, voting district, constituency, riding, ward, division, or (election) precinct is a subdivision of a larger State (polity), state (a country, administrative region, ...
),
agreement Agreement may refer to: Agreements between people and organizations * Gentlemen's agreement, not enforceable by law * Trade agreement, between countries * Consensus, a decision-making process * Contract, enforceable in a court of law ** Meeting o ...
, the nature of crosslinguistic variation, and the relationship between form and meaning (
semantics Semantics (from grc, σημαντικός ''sēmantikós'', "significant") is the study of reference, meaning, or truth. The term can be used to refer to subfields of several distinct disciplines, including philosophy Philosophy (f ...
). There are numerous approaches to syntax that differ in their central assumptions and goals.


Etymology

The word ''syntax'' comes from
Ancient Greek Ancient Greek includes the forms of the Greek language used in ancient Greece and the ancient world from around 1500 BC to 300 BC. It is often roughly divided into the following periods: Mycenaean Greek (), Dark Ages (), the Archaic peri ...
roots: "coordination", which consists of ''syn'', "together", and ''táxis'', "ordering".


Topics

The field of syntax contains a number of various topics that a syntactic theory is often designed to handle. The relation between the topics is treated differently in different theories, and some of them may not be considered to be distinct but instead to be derived from one another (i.e. word order can be seen as the result of movement rules derived from grammatical relations).


Sequencing of subject, verb, and object

One basic description of a language's syntax is the sequence in which the
subject Subject ( la, subiectus "lying beneath") may refer to: Philosophy *''Hypokeimenon'', or ''subiectum'', in metaphysics, the "internal", non-objective being of a thing **Subject (philosophy), a being that has subjective experiences, subjective cons ...
(S),
verb A verb () is a word (part of speech) that in syntax generally conveys an action (''bring'', ''read'', ''walk'', ''run'', ''learn''), an occurrence (''happen'', ''become''), or a state of being (''be'', ''exist'', ''stand''). In the usual descri ...
(V), and
object Object may refer to: General meanings * Object (philosophy), a thing, being, or concept ** Object (abstract), an object which does not exist at any particular time or place ** Physical object, an identifiable collection of matter * Goal, an ai ...
(O) usually appear in sentences. Over 85% of languages usually place the subject first, either in the sequence SVO or the sequence SOV. The other possible sequences are VSO, VOS, OVS, and OSV, the last three of which are rare. In most generative theories of syntax, the surface differences arise from a more complex clausal phrase structure, and each order may be compatible with multiple derivations. However, word order can also reflect the semantics or function of the ordered elements.


Grammatical relations

Another description of a language considers the set of possible grammatical relations in a language or in general and how they behave in relation to one another in the
morphosyntactic alignment In linguistics, morphosyntactic alignment is the grammatical relationship between Argument (linguistics), arguments—specifically, between the two arguments (in English, subject and object) of transitive verbs like ''the dog chased the cat'', an ...
of the language. The description of grammatical relations can also reflect transitivity,
passivization A passive voice construction is a grammatical voice construction that is found in many languages. In a clause with passive voice, the grammatical subject expresses the ''theme'' or ''patient'' of the main verb – that is, the person or thing t ...
, and head-dependent-marking or other agreement. Languages have different criteria for grammatical relations. For example, subjecthood criteria may have implications for how the subject is referred to from a relative clause or coreferential with an element in an infinite clause.


Constituency

Constituency is the feature of being a
constituent Constituent or constituency may refer to: Politics * An individual voter within an electoral district, state, community, or organization * Advocacy group or constituency * Constituent assembly * Constituencies of Namibia Other meanings * Const ...
and how words can work together to form a constituent (or ''phrase''). Constituents are often moved as units, and the constituent can be the domain of agreement. Some languages allow discontinuous phrases in which words belonging to the same constituent are not immediately adjacent but are broken up by other constituents. Constituents may be recursive, as they may consist of other constituents, potentially of the same type.


Early history

The ''
Aṣṭādhyāyī The (Devanagari अष्टाध्यायी) is a grammar that describes a form of an early Indo-Aryan language: Sanskrit. Authored by Sanskrit philologist and scholar Pāṇini and dated to around 500 BCE, it describes the language as cu ...
'' of
Pāṇini , era = ;;6th–5th century BCE , region = Indian philosophy , main_interests = Grammar, linguistics , notable_works = ' (Sanskrit#Classical Sanskrit, Classical Sanskrit) , influenced= , notable_ideas=Descript ...
, from c. 4th century BC in
Ancient India According to consensus in modern genetics, anatomically modern humans first arrived on the Indian subcontinent from Africa between 73,000 and 55,000 years ago. Quote: "Y-Chromosome and Mt-DNA data support the colonization of South Asia by m ...
, is often cited as an example of a premodern work that approaches the sophistication of a modern syntactic theory since works on
grammar In linguistics, the grammar of a natural language is its set of structure, structural constraints on speakers' or writers' composition of clause (linguistics), clauses, phrases, and words. The term can also refer to the study of such constraint ...
had been written long before modern syntax came about. In the West, the school of thought that came to be known as "traditional grammar" began with the work of
Dionysius Thrax Dionysius Thrax ( grc-gre, Διονύσιος ὁ Θρᾷξ ''Dionýsios ho Thrâix'', 170–90 BC) was a Greek grammarian and a pupil of Aristarchus of Samothrace. He was long considered to be the author of the earliest grammatical text on the G ...
. For centuries, a framework known as , first expounded in 1660 by
Antoine Arnauld Antoine Arnauld (6 February 16128 August 1694) was a French Catholic theologian, philosopher and mathematician. He was one of the leading intellectuals of the Jansenist group of Port-Royal and had a very thorough knowledge of patristics. Contem ...
in a book of the same title, dominated work in syntax: as its basic premise the assumption that language is a direct reflection of thought processes and so there is a single most natural way to express a thought. However, in the 19th century, with the development of historical-comparative linguistics, linguists began to realize the sheer diversity of human language and to question fundamental assumptions about the relationship between language and logic. It became apparent that there was no such thing as the most natural way to express a thought and so logic could no longer be relied upon as a basis for studying the structure of language. The Port-Royal-des-Champs, Port-Royal grammar modeled the study of syntax upon that of logic. (Indeed, large parts of Port-Royal Logic were copied or adapted from the ''Grammaire générale''.) Syntactic categories were identified with logical ones, and all sentences were analyzed in terms of "subject – copula – predicate". Initially, that view was adopted even by the early comparative linguists such as Franz Bopp. The central role of syntax within theoretical linguistics became clear only in the 20th century, which could reasonably be called the "century of syntactic theory" as far as linguistics is concerned. (For a detailed and critical survey of the history of syntax in the last two centuries, see the monumental work by Giorgio Graffi (2001).)


Theories

There are a number of theoretical approaches to the discipline of syntax. One school of thought, founded in the works of Derek Bickerton, sees syntax as a branch of biology, since it conceives of syntax as the study of linguistic knowledge as embodied in the human mind. Other linguists (e.g., Gerald Gazdar) take a more Philosophy of mathematics#Platonism, Platonistic view since they regard syntax to be the study of an abstract formal system. Yet others (e.g., Joseph Greenberg) consider syntax a taxonomical device to reach broad generalizations across languages. Syntacticians have attempted to explain the causes of word-order variation within individual languages and cross-linguistically. Much of such work has been done within the framework of generative grammar, which holds that syntax depends on a universal grammar, genetic endowment common to the human species. In that framework and in others, linguistic typology and linguistic universal, universals have been primary explicanda. Alternative explanations, such as those by functional linguistics, functional linguists, have been sought in language processing in the brain, language processing. It is suggested that the brain finds it easier to parsing, parse syntactic tree diagram, syntactic patterns that are either right- or left-branching (linguistics), branching but not mixed. The most-widely held approach is the performance–grammar correspondence hypothesis by John A. Hawkins (linguist), John A. Hawkins, who suggests that language is a non-innate adaptive system, adaptation to innate Cognition, cognitive mechanisms. Cross-linguistic tendencies are considered as being based on language users' preference for grammars that are organized efficiently and on their avoidance of word orderings that cause processing difficulty. Some languages, however, exhibit regular inefficient patterning such as the VO languages Chinese language, Chinese, with the adpositional phrase before the verb, and Finnish grammar, Finnish, which has postpositions, but there are few other profoundly exceptional languages. More recently, it is suggested that the left- versus right-branching patterns are cross-linguistically related only to the place of role-marking connectives (adpositions and subordinator (grammar), subordinators), which links the phenomena with the semantic mapping of sentences.


Theoretical syntactic models


Dependency grammar

Dependency grammar is an approach to sentence structure in which syntactic units are arranged according to the dependency relation, as opposed to the constituency relation of phrase structure grammars. Dependencies are directed links between words. The (finite) verb is seen as the root of all clause structure and all the other words in the clause are either directly or indirectly dependent on the root. Some prominent dependency-based theories of syntax are the following: * Recursive categorical syntax, or algebraic syntax * Functional generative description * Meaning–text theory * Operator grammar * Word grammar Lucien Tesnière (1893–1954) is widely seen as the father of modern dependency-based theories of syntax and grammar. He argued vehemently against the binary division of the clause into
subject Subject ( la, subiectus "lying beneath") may refer to: Philosophy *''Hypokeimenon'', or ''subiectum'', in metaphysics, the "internal", non-objective being of a thing **Subject (philosophy), a being that has subjective experiences, subjective cons ...
and predicate (grammar), predicate that is associated with the grammars of his day (S → NP VP) and remains at the core of most phrase structure grammars. In the place of that division, he positioned the verb as the root of all clause structure.


Categorial grammar

Categorial grammar is an approach in which constituents combine as function (math), function and argument of a function, argument, according to combinatory possibilities specified in their syntactic categories. For example, other approaches might posit a rule that combines a noun phrase (NP) and a verb phrase (VP), but CG would posit a syntactic category ''NP'' and another ''NP\S'', read as "a category that searches to the left (indicated by \) for an NP (the element on the left) and outputs a sentence (the element on the right)." Thus, the syntactic category for an intransitive verb is a complex formula representing the fact that the verb acts as a function word requiring an NP as an input and produces a sentence level structure as an output. The complex category is notated as (NP\S) instead of V. The category of transitive verb is defined as an element that requires two NPs (its subject and its direct object) to form a sentence. That is notated as (NP/(NP\S)), which means, "A category that searches to the right (indicated by /) for an NP (the object) and generates a function (equivalent to the VP) which is (NP\S), which in turn represents a function that searches to the left for an NP and produces a sentence." Tree-adjoining grammar is a categorial grammar that adds in partial tree structures to the categories.


Stochastic/probabilistic grammars/network theories

Theoretical approaches to syntax that are based upon probability theory are known as stochastic grammars. One common implementation of such an approach makes use of a neural network or connectionism.


Functional grammars

Functionalist models of grammar study the form–function interaction by performing a structural and a functional analysis. * Functional discourse grammar (Dik) * Prague linguistic circle * Role and reference grammar (RRG) * Systemic functional grammar


Generative syntax

Generative syntax is the study of syntax within the overarching framework of generative grammar. Generative theories of syntax typically propose analyses of grammatical patterns using formal tools such as phrase structure grammars augmented with additional operations such as syntactic movement. Their goal in analyzing a particular language is to specify rules which generate all and only the expressions which are grammaticality, well-formed in that language. In doing so, they seek to identify innate domain-specific principles of linguistic cognition, in line with the wider goals of the generative enterprise. Generative syntax is among the approaches that adopt the principle of the autonomy of syntax by assuming that meaning and communicative intent is determined by the syntax, rather than the other way around. Generative syntax was proposed in the late 1950s by Noam Chomsky, building on earlier work by Zellig Harris, Louis Hjelmslev, and others. Since then, numerous theories have been proposed under its umbrella: * Transformational grammar (TG) (Original theory of generative syntax laid out by Chomsky in ''Syntactic Structures'' in 1957) * Government and binding theory (GB) (revised theory in the tradition of TG developed mainly by Chomsky in the 1970s and 1980s) * Minimalist program (MP) (a reworking of the theory out of the GB framework published by Chomsky in 1995) Other theories that find their origin in the generative paradigm are: * Arc pair grammar * Generalized phrase structure grammar (GPSG) * Generative semantics * Head-driven phrase structure grammar (HPSG) * Lexical functional grammar (LFG) * Nanosyntax * Relational grammar (RG) * Harmonic grammar (HG)


Cognitive and usage-based grammars

The Cognitive Linguistics framework stems from generative grammar but adheres to evolutionary linguistics, evolutionary, rather than Noam Chomsky, Chomskyan, linguistics. Cognitive models often recognise the generative assumption that the object belongs to the verb phrase. Cognitive frameworks include the following: * Cognitive grammar * Construction grammar (CxG) * Emergent grammar


See also

* Cartographic syntax * List of syntactic phenomena * Metasyntax * Musical syntax * Semiotics * Syntactic category * ''Syntax (journal), Syntax'' (academic journal) * Syntax (programming languages) * Syntax–Semantics Interface * Usage


Syntactic terms

* Adjective * Adjective phrase * Adjunct (grammar), Adjunct * Adpositional phrase * Adverb * Anaphora (linguistics), Anaphora * Answer ellipsis * Antecedent (grammar), Antecedent * Antecedent-contained deletion * Appositive * Argument (linguistics), Argument * Article (grammar), Article * Grammatical aspect, Aspect * Attributive adjective and predicative adjective * Auxiliary verb * Binding (linguistics), Binding * Branching (linguistics), Branching * c-command * Grammatical case, Case * Syntactic category, Category * Catena (linguistics), Catena * Clause * Closed class word * Comparison (grammar), Comparative * Complement (linguistics), Complement * Compound (linguistics), Compound noun and adjective * Grammatical conjugation, Conjugation * Conjunction (grammar), Conjunction * Constituent (linguistics), Constituent * Coordination (linguistics), Coordination * Coreference * Crossover effects, Crossover * Dangling modifier * Declension * Dependency grammar * Dependent-marking language, Dependent marking * Determiner (linguistics), Determiner * Discontinuity (linguistics), Discontinuity * Do-support * Dual (grammatical number), Dual (form for two) * Ellipsis * Endocentric * Exceptional case-marking * Expletive (linguistics), Expletive * Extraposition * Finite verb * Function word * Gapping * Grammatical gender, Gender * Gerund * Government (linguistics), Government * Head (linguistics), Head * Head-marking language, Head marking * Infinitive * Inverse copular constructions, Inverse copular construction * Inversion (linguistics), Inversion * Lexical item * Logical form (linguistics) * m-command * Measure word (classifier) * Merge (linguistics), Merge * Modal particle * Modal verb * Grammatical modifier, Modifier * Grammatical mood, Mood * Syntactic movement, Movement * Movement paradox * Nanosyntax * Negative inversion * Non-configurational language * Non-finite verb * Noun * Noun ellipsis * Noun phrase * Grammatical number, Number * Object (grammar), Object * Open class (linguistics), Open class word * Parasitic gap * Part of speech * Grammatical particle, Particle * Periphrasis * Grammatical person, Person * Personal pronoun * Pied-piping * Phrasal verb * Phrase * Phrase structure grammar * Plural * Predicate (grammar), Predicate * Predicative expression * Preposition and postposition * Pronoun * Pseudogapping * Raising (linguistics), Raising * Grammatical relation * Restrictiveness * Right node raising * Sandhi * Scrambling (linguistics), Scrambling * Selection (linguistics), Selection * Sentence (linguistics), Sentence * Separable verb * Shifting (syntax), Shifting * Grammatical number, Singular * Sluicing * Small clause * Stripping (linguistics), Stripping * Subcategorization * Subject (grammar), Subject * Subject-auxiliary inversion * Subject-verb inversion * Subordination (linguistics), Subordination * Superlative * Grammatical tense, Tense * Topicalization * Tough movement * Uninflected word * V2 word order * Valency (linguistics), Valency * Verb * Verb phrase * Verb phrase ellipsis * Voice (grammar), Voice * Wh-movement * Word order * X-bar theory


References


Citations


Sources

* * * * * An interdisciplinary essay on the interplay between logic and linguistics on syntactic theories. *


Further reading

* 5 Volumes; 77 case studies of syntactic phenomena. * * Attempts to be a theory-neutral introduction. The companion surveys the major theories. Jointly reviewed in * * part II: Computational approaches to syntax.


External links


The syntax of natural language: An online introduction using the Trees program
eatrice Santorini & Anthony Kroch, University of Pennsylvania, 2007 {{Authority control Syntax, Syntactic entities, Grammar Language Branches of linguistics Philosophy of language Semiotics Linguistics terminology, +