Definition
The term "consonant gradation" refers to a word-medial alternation of consonants between fortis and lenis realisations. The fortis strong grade appears in historically open syllables (ending in a vowel), while the lenis weak grade appears in historically closed syllables (ending in a consonant). The exact realisation of the fortis–lenis distinction differs between the branches. In the Samic languages it was realised through fortition, specifically lengthening, in the strong grade. In the Finnic and Samoyedic languages, there was instead lenition in the weak grade. Thus, the exact realization of the contrast is not crucial. The language groups differ in regard to their treatment sequences of a vowel followed by ''j'' or ''w'' in Proto-Uralic. In the Samic languages, the second part of these remains phonologically a consonant, and can thus close the syllable before it, triggering the weak grade. It also takes part in gradation itself, lengthening in the strong grade. In Finnic, on the other hand, these were treated as diphthongs, and were equivalent to long vowels in terms of syllable structure. Consequently, they did not close the syllable and did not affect gradation. Consonant gradation is understood to have originally been a predictable phonological process. In all languages that retain it, however, it has evolved further to a less predictable system of consonant mutation, ofHistorical connections
There is no consensus view on the ultimate origin of consonant gradation in the Uralic languages. Three broad positions may be distinguished: * Gradation in Finnic, Samic and Samoyedic are all connected to one another. * Gradation in Finnic and Samic are connected; gradation in Samoyedic is an unrelated phenomenon. * There is no connection between gradation in any of the three language groups, and the similarities are accidental. In all three groups, consonant gradation has the same conditioning, the distinction between open and closed syllables. In this light, and in the absence of any evidence of the same system having existed in any unrelated language in the world, Helimski (1995) has argued that the latter two options should be rejected as implausible. If a connection exists, it is also disputed what its nature may be, again allowing for three broad positions: * Gradation is common inheritance (from either Proto-Uralic or Proto-Finno-Samic). * Gradation is an areal phenomenon that has developed throughFinnic languages
The original effect of gradation in the Finnic languages can be reconstructed as a lenition of the consonant at the beginning of a closed syllable. Lenition resulted in geminate (long) stops and affricates being shortened, and in short voiceless obstruents /*p *t *k/ becoming voiced, while short voiced obstruents /*b *d *g/ became fricatives: * *pp → *p̆p * *tt → *t̆t * *cc → *c̆c * *kk → *k̆k * *p → *b * *t → *d * *k → *g **b → *β **d → *ð **g → *ɣ Only stops and affricates were affected, not other consonants. Moreover, only the last member of a consonant cluster was subject to gradation, and single stops and affricates were only affected if they were not adjacent to another obstruent. Thus, two-obstruent combinations like ''kt'', ''st'' and ''tk'' did not undergo lenition, nor did obstruent-sonorant combinations like ''kl'' and ''tr''. The voiced stops ''*b'' ''*d'' ''*g'' generally lenited to fricatives unless they were preceded by a nasal. This change may have occurred already in Proto-Finnic, but is not found in Livonian and Veps. The fricatives later underwent further changes, and the dental and velar fricatives have been lost altogether in most Finnic varieties. The weakened grades of geminate consonants did not merge with the strong grades of the singleton consonants in Proto-Finnic, and still counted as geminates for the purposes of syllabification. There remained for a period an intermediate quantity, half-long *''-t̆t-'', which still closed the preceding syllable. Consequently, a syllable ending with a geminate in the weak grade still triggered a weak grade on the preceding syllable as well. In Finnish, the half-long consonants eventually merged with the strong-grade singleton consonants, but in most other Finnic languages, the strong-grade singletons underwent a secondary lenition which prevented this merger. Gradation later expanded to include a pattern ''*s'' ~ ''*h'', presumed to reflect a former pattern ''*s'' ~ ''*z''. This type of gradation only systematically appears in cases of word-final ''*s'', which between vowels uniformly becomes ''*h'': Finnish 'bush' has the genitive < . An example is also found after a stressed syllable, however, in the exceptional monosyllabic root ''*mees'' : ''*meehe-'' "man"; and in a fossilized form, in the postpositions ''lähellä'' "near" vs. ''läsnä'' "present", reflecting the adessive and the essive of a root ''*läse-'' "vicinity". In cases of root-internal ''*s'', this pattern is not normally found (e.g. Finnish ''pesä'' 'nest' : plural ''pesät''), though Votic later reintroduced a gradation pattern : here (''pezäd''). Veps and Livonian have largely leveled the original gradation system, and reflect both weak and strong grades of single stops as ; this may be an archaism or a substitution of voiced stops for fricatives due to foreign influence (Russian for Veps, Latvian for Livonian). Except for northernmost Veps dialects, both grades of geminate stops are also reflected as .Finnish
Finnish consonant gradation generally preserves the Proto-Finnic pattern fairly well. The conditioning of syllable structure is still visible in most cases, but it is no longer productive: gradation has become a grammatical feature.Historical sound changes affecting realization of weak grades
* The weak grades *p̆p, *t̆t, *k̆k of geminates coincided with plain *p, *t, *k. * The weak grades *mb, *nd, *ŋg of nasal+stop clusters were assimilated to geminate nasals , , (''ng''). * The weak grades *lð, *rð of liquid+ clusters were similarly assimilated to geminate liquids , . * *β merged with *ʋ (''v''). This may have been lost later. For example, the 3rd person singular suffix *-pi is represented by aAnalogical extension of gradation
The consonant clusters and were, comprising two obstruents, not originally subject to gradation (as is still the case for similar clusters such as , , ). However, gradation pairs ''ht : *hð'' and ''hk : *hɣ'' were at one point introduced. The first of these patterns remains common in modern Finnish, e.g. ''vahti : vahdit'' ''. The second is only found in a limited number of words, e.g. ''pohje : pohkeet'' 'calf : calves', but ''rahka : rahkat'' ''. Usage varies for some words with , e.g. for the plural of ''nahka'' 'leather, hide', both ''nahat'' and ''nahkat'' are acceptable. Quantitative consonant gradation has expanded to include in addition to the pairs ''kk : k'', ''pp : p'', ''tt : t'', also ''gg : g'' and ''bb : b'' (but not ''dd : d'') in a number of recent loanwords, such as 'to blog'; 'to lobby'.Historical sound changes affecting conditions of gradation
One important change was the loss of word-final *-k and *-h early on in the history of Finnish. This resulted in many open syllables with weak grades. In particular, the majority of nouns ending in ''-e'' are affected by this, with a weak grade in the nominative form. The imperative form of verbs also ended in a now-lost ''-k''. For examples, ''side'' 'bandage', from *''siðe'', earlier *''siðek'' (cf. Veps ''sideg'', Eastern Votic ''sidõg''); 'to get' → ''hae!'' 'get! (imp.)' from *''haɣe'', earlier *''haɣek''. Traces of the original syllable closure can be seen in sandhi effects: these classes of words can still be analyzed to contain the assimilative word-final 'consonant' ˣ, realized as lengthening of the next word's initial consonant. Therefore, ''hae side varastosta'' 'get a bandage from storage!' is pronounced , where the weak grades indeed occur in closed syllables. The loss of ''-k'' combined with loss of ''d'' gave rise to the modern Finnish infinitive ending, which was historically *''-tak/täk''. The final *''-k'' triggered gradation, so that the ending normally became *''-dak/däk''. In turn, following the loss of ''d'' between unstressed vowels, and the loss of final *''-k'' only *''-aˣ/äˣ'' remained. Thus, (originally *) has only ''-a'' as the ''d'' was lost. But in verbs like ''juo-da'' 'to drink' the /d/ remained since it followed a stressed syllable. In the case of verbs like ''tulla'' 'to come', the earlier form was *''tul-ðak'', but the ''*ð'' was assimilated to the /l/ according to the patterns described above. The original strong grade was preserved in verbs like ''hais-ta'' 'to stink' since gradation did not take place when a consonant followed /s/. The situation appears differently in the many verbs ending in ''-ata/ätä''. These verbs seem to have preserved the strong grade in the infinitive ending, going counter to the rules of gradation. However, historically it is in fact a weak grade: the stem of the verb itself ended in *''-at/ät-'', and this is still visible in the 3rd person imperative ending ''-atkoon/ätköön''. Thus, when combined with the infinitive ending, the verb ended in *''-attak/ättäk'' (similar to the origin of the ''-ton/tön'' suffix described above). The ''-k'' then weakened the consonant from a geminate *''-tt-'' to a single *''-t-'', and later loss of ''-k'' resulted in the final form ''-ata/ätä''. However, even though this is now a single consonant, it was originally a geminate and therefore triggers the weak grade on the syllable before it. So whereas the infinitive may be for example ''hypätä'' 'to jump', its original stem was *''hyppät-'', as can be seen in the first-person singular form ''hyppään'' 'I jump', from earlier *''hyppäðen'' with loss of ''*-ð-''. An opposite effect was caused by the loss of *''h'' and *''ð'' between unstressed vowels. Loss of ''h'' affected nouns and adjectives ending in *''-s'' or *''-h'', such as ''kuningas'' 'king'. In the nominative, this ''-s'' appeared as usual, and as the preceding syllable was closed, the weak grade ''ng'' appeared. But when a case ending such as the genitive was added, the result was originally *''kuninkasen'', which was then weakened to *''kuninkahen'', and the loss of ''-h-'' then resulted in the modern form ''kuninkaan''. The intermediate steps are seen in ''mies'' 'man'. Here, following a stressed syllable, the ''-h-'' was not lost, so that its genitive is ''miehen''. Similar changes affected the illative ending, which was ''-hVn'' where V was the same as the vowel preceding the ending. The ''h'' is preserved after stressed syllables, as in ''maahan'' 'into the land' (from ''maa''), but lost otherwise as in ''kotiin'' 'into the home' (from earlier *''kotihin'', from ''koti''). This explains why ''kotiin'' retains a strong grade even though a closed syllable follows it. The Pohjanmaa dialect of Finnish retains the ''-h-'', however. Words that now end in ''-e'' are in fact very similar to those ending in ''-s''. These originally ended with ''-k'' or ''-h'' so that the nominative ended in a consonant just as ''kuningas'' and therefore the preceding syllable was in the weak grade. But after an ending was added, the weak grade ''g'' appeared, which eventually disappeared just as ''h'' did.Analogical limitation of gradation
While syllabic gradation remains generally productive, the distortions of its original phonetic conditions have left it essentially a morphologically conditioned process. This is particularly visible in forms that display a strong grade where a weak would be historically expected, or vice versa. Possessive suffixes, in particular, are always preceded by the strong grade, even if the suffix may cause the syllable to be closed. For example, 'our bed' is ''sänkymme'', not ''ˣsängymme''. Strong grades may also be found in closed syllables in contractions such as ''jotta en'' → ''jotten''. Several recent loans and coinages with simple are also left entirely outside of gradation, e.g. ''auto'' (: ''auton'') 'car', ''eka'' (: ''ekan'') 'first', ''muki'' (: ''mukin'') 'mug', ''peti'' (: ''petin'', sometimes ''pedin'' ) 'bed', ''söpö'' (: ''söpön'') 'cute'. A number of proper names such as Alepa, Arto,Karelian
Karelian consonant gradation is quite similar to Finnish: *β *ð *ɣ have been lost in a fashion essentially identical to Eastern Finnish (and may have occurred in the common ancestor of the two), with the exception that assimilation rather than loss has occurred also for *lɣ and *rɣ. E.g. the plural of ''jalka'' 'foot' is ''jallat'', contrasting with ''jalat'' in Finnish and ''jalad'' in Estonian. Karelian still includes some gradation pairs which Finnish does not. The consonants undergo consonant gradation when following a coronal obstruent : ''muistua'' 'to remember' → ''muissan'' 'I remember', ''matka'' → ''matan'' 'trip' (nom. → gen.). This development may be by analogy of the corresponding liquid clusters. On the other hand, some Karelian dialects (such as Livvi or Olonets) do not allow for gradation in clusters beginning on nasals. Thus, the Olonets Karelian equivalent of Finnish ''vanhemmat'' (cf. ''vanhempi'' 'older') is ''vahnembat''. The Karelian phoneme inventory also includes the affricate (represented in the orthography as ''č''), which may be found geminated and is such subject to quantitative gradation: ''meččä'' 'forest' → ''mečäššä'' 'in (the) forest'.Votic
Votic has two quantities for consonants and vowels, which basically match up with the Finnish counterparts. The Votic phoneme inventory includes a set of fully voiced stops, which Paul Ariste (''A Grammar of the Votic Language'') describes as being the same as in Russian. Thus, in addition to quantitative alternations between and , Votic also has a system of qualitative alternations in which the distinguishing feature is ''voicing'', and so the voiceless stops are known to alternate with . As in Estonian, Karelian, and Eastern dialects of Finnish, the weak grade *ð of in inherited vocabulary has been lost or assimilated to adjacent sounds in Votic; the weak grade *β of has similarly become , or assimilated to in the cluster . However, the weak grade of survives, as before a back vowel or before a front vowel. A noticeable feature of Votic is that gradation has been extended to several consonant clusters that were not originally affected. As in Finnish, this includes the clusters and with a voicing-neutral first member, but also further clusters, even several ones introduced only in Russian loans. The alternations involving the voiced affricate dž are only found in the Eastern dialects. In the Western dialects, there are several possible weak grade counterparts of tš: Further minor variation in these gradation patterns was found down to the level of individual villages. Votic also has a number of alternations betweenEstonian
Though otherwise closely related to Votic, consonant gradation in Estonian is quite different from the other Finnic languages. One extremely important difference is the existence of three grades of consonants (alternations like strong grade ''pada'' 'pot (nom.)', weak grade ''paja'' 'pot (gen.)', overlong grade ''patta'' 'pot (ill.)'). This can be said to generally correlate with the existence of three degrees of consonant length (e.g. ''d'', ''t'', and ''tt''), but since the alternation ''d'' ~ ''t'' occurs only after heavy syllables, and the alternations ''d'' ~ ''tt'' and ''t'' ~ ''tt'' occur only after light syllables, there is no single paradigm that has this simple alternation. However, weak grades like ''v'', ''j'', or ''∅'' that alternate with stops like ''b'', ''d'', or ''g'' originate from the weak grade of these stops, and these may still synchronically alternate with the over-long grades (''pp'', ''tt'', ''kk'') within the same paradigm, giving paradigms with three underlying grades. Another extremely important feature of Estonian gradation is that, due to the greater loss of word-final segments (both consonants and vowels), the Estonian gradation is an almost entirely opaque process, where the consonant grade (short, long, or overlong) must be listed for each class of wordform. So, for example, 'embrace' has the same form for all cases (e.g. genitive ), while ''hammas'' 'tooth' has weak grade ''mm'' in the nominative ''hammas'' and partitive ''hammast'', but strong form ''mb'' in the genitive ''hamba'' and all other cases of the singular. There is a large number of cases in which inflectional endings are identical except for how they affect the consonant grade, e.g. ''leht'' 'leaf' belongs to a declension class in which both the genitive and the partitive singular are formed by adding ''-e'', but the genitive takes the weak form (''leh-e''), while the partitive takes the strong form (''leht-e''). In the end, the types of generalizations that can be made are that some inflectional categories always take the strong form (e.g. partitive plural, ''-ma'' infinitive), some always take the weak form (e.g. ''-tud'' participle), some forms may take the overlong form (some partitive singulars, short illative singular), while other inflectional categories are underdetermined for whether they occur with weak or strong grade. In this last case, within a paradigm some forms are constrained to have the same grade and others are constrained to have the opposite grade; thus all present tense forms for the same verb have the same grade, though some verbs have strong (''hakkan'' 'I begin', ''hakkad'' 'you begin', etc.) and others have weak ( 'I read', , 'you read', etc.), and the ''-da'' infinitive has the opposite grade from the present (''hakata'' 'to begin', ''lugeda'' 'to read'). The system of gradation has also expanded to include gradation of all consonant clusters and geminate consonants (generally quantitative), when occurring after short vowels, and vowel gradation between long and overlong vowels, although these are not written except for the distinction between voiceless stops and geminate voiceless stops (e.g. overlong strong grade ''tt'' with weak grade ''t''). E.g. ''linn'' , 'city (nom.)' vs. ''linna'' 'city (gen.)'. In consonant clusters, in the strong grade the first consonant is lengthened, e.g. ''must'' , 'black (nom.)' vs. ''musta'' 'black (gen.)'. Before single consonants, long vowels and diphthongs also become overlong in strong forms and remain merely long in weak forms, e.g. ''kool'' , 'school (nom.)' vs. ''kooli'' 'school (gen.)'.Samic languages
Gradation was present in Proto-Samic, and is inherited in most Samic languages. It is different from the gradation found in the Finnic languages in some important aspects: * Gradation applies to all consonants, even consonant clusters. * Geminate stops and affricates were realised with preaspiration in Proto-Samic, and were thus phonetically distinct from singletons in more than length alone. * Rather than featuring lenition in the weak grade, the weak grade generally reflects the original consonant. Instead, it is the strong grade that was modified: single consonants were lengthened into half-long in the strong grade, geminate consonants were lengthened to overlong. * It only applies in consonants at the end of a stressed syllable; consonants following unstressed syllables remain unlengthened and do not show grade alternations. Similar to the cases of Veps and Livonian within Finnic, the Southern Sami language at the westernmost end of the Sami language continuum has lost all gradation. In the remaining Sami languages, the strong grade of the singletons merged with the weak grade of geminates, creating a three-quantity distinction between short, long and overlong consonants. In Kildin and Ter Sami, this merger did not affect stops and affricates, due to the additional preaspiration found on original geminates. In the others, the merger affected stops and affricates as well, with the strong grade of singletons receiving secondary preaspiration. In the Western Samic languages, geminate nasals became pre-stopped, which affected the strong grade of singletons as well (outside Southern Sami) due to the historical merger of these grades. In the languages in closest contact to Finnic ( Northern, Inari and Skolt), a number of developments towards the situation in Finnish and Karelian have occurred, such as the change of unlengthened ''*t'' to .Northern Sami
Northern Sami has a system of three phonological lengths for consonants, and thus has extensive sets of alternations. Quantity 3 is represented as lengthening of the coda part of a geminate or cluster, which is absent in quantity 2. Quantity 1 consists of only an onset consonant, with the preceding syllable having no coda. In addition, most dialects of Northern Sami feature ''coda maximisation'', which geminates the last member of a cluster in various environments (most commonly in two-consonant clusters of quantity 2, in which the first member is voiced). Most sonorants and fricatives are only subject to quantitative gradation, but nasals, stops, affricates and the glide are subject to both quantitative and qualitative changes. Some words alternate between three grades, though not all words do. Note that the following apostrophe marking the over-long grade is not used in the official orthography, although it is generally found in dictionaries. Some gradation triads include the following:Samoyedic languages
Nganasan
Nganasan, alone of the Samoyedic languages (or indeed any Uralic languages east of Finnic), shows systematic qualitative gradation of stops and fricatives. Gradation occurs in intervocalic position as well as in consonant clusters consisting of a nasal and a stop. Note that /h/ and /ŋh/ descend from historical /p/ and /mp/, respectively. Examples of Nganasan consonant gradation can be seen in the table below. The first form given is always theSelkup
A limited form of consonant gradation is found in the Ket dialect of Selkup. In certain environments, geminate stops can alternate with short (allophonically voiced) ones, under the usual conditions for radical gradation. E.g.:See also
* Lenition * Grammatischer Wechsel * Kluge's Law * Consonant harmonyNotes
References
*Helimski, Eugene 1998. Nganasan. In: Daniel Abondolo (ed.), ''The Uralic Languages'', pp. 480–515. London / New York: Routledge.External links