HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In
linguistics Linguistics is the science, scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure ...
, age-graded variation is differences in speech habits within a
community A community is a social unit (a group of living things) with commonality such as place, norms, religion, values, customs, or identity. Communities may share a sense of place situated in a given geographical area (e.g. a country, village, ...
that are associated with age. Age-grading occurs when individuals change their linguistic behavior throughout their lifetimes, but the community as a whole does not change.


Definition

The term “age-grading” was first introduced by anthropologist/linguist
Charles F. Hockett Charles Francis Hockett (January 17, 1916 – November 3, 2000) was an American linguist who developed many influential ideas in American structuralism#Structuralism in linguistics, structuralist linguistics. He represents the post-Leonard Bloomfi ...
, but has been defined as it is used today by sociolinguist
William Labov William Labov ( ; born December 4, 1927) is an American linguist widely regarded as the founder of the discipline of variationist sociolinguistics. He has been described as "an enormously original and influential figure who has created much of ...
, who identified it as an individual linguistic change against a backdrop of community stability. Age-grading is not limited to changes at any one particular stage in life, but can be studied in the way that babies, adolescents, young adults speak, and the elderly speak.Trudgill, Peter. ''Sociolinguistics: An Introduction to Language and Society''. London: Penguin, 2000. Print. Age-grading involves the use of stable
sociolinguistic Sociolinguistics is the descriptive study of the effect of any or all aspects of society, including cultural norms, expectations, and context, on the way language is used, and society's effect on language. It can overlap with the sociology of l ...
variables, or features that are not currently undergoing any linguistic change. Linguistic features that are age-graded typically are stigmatized or have a high degree of social awareness and can be consciously controlled.Labov, William. ''Principles of Linguistic Change''. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1994. Print. For this reason, age-graded variables tend to be semantic (e.g., an entire word) as opposed to phonetic. Age-graded changes recur at a particular age in successive generations and are regular and predictable changes. These changes are thought to mark developmental/maturational stages in the individual’s life.Chambers, J. K. ''Sociolinguistic Theory: Linguistic Variation and Its Social Significance'' (pp 200-209. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1995. Print.


The classic age-grading pattern

The expected pattern is a u- or v-shaped curve (see figure to the right). Nonprestigious age-graded linguistic features tend to peak during adolescence “when peer group pressure not to conform to society’s norms is greatest” (Holmes 1992:184).Holmes, Janet. ''An Introduction to Sociolinguistics'' (pp 173-80, 216-17, 237-39). London: Longman, 1992. Print. The use of standard or prestige forms peaks between the ages of 30 and 55. When people reach middle age, they tend to follow societal norms more because that is when the societal pressure to conform is greatest. At this stage of life, people become more conservative and use the fewest vernacular forms. Nonprestigious forms may resurface in old age when people are out of the workforce and social pressures are reduced.


Age-grading and the apparent-time hypothesis

Age-graded variation stands in contrast to the
apparent-time hypothesis The apparent-time hypothesis is a methodological construct in sociolinguistics whereby language change is studied by comparing the speech of individuals of different ages. If language change is taking place, the apparent-time hypothesis assumes that ...
, which assumes that an individual’s vernacular does not change once they are past adolescence. According to the apparent-time hypothesis, age-stratified variation is often indicative of a linguistic change in progress. For example, if in a survey of a population, patterned differences between the speech of individuals 75 years old, 50 years old, and 25 years old may indicate changes that have occurred over the past 50 years.Bailey, Guy. ''Real and Apparent Time''. In J.K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill, & Natalie Schilling-Estes d ''The Handbook of Language Variation and Change'' (pp 312-332). Oxford, England: Blackwell, 2002. Print. This presents a problem when studying age-graded variation because age-grading relies on individuals changing their speech patterns throughout their lives. The applicability of age-graded variation should be confirmed by
real-time Real-time or real time describes various operations in computing or other processes that must guarantee response times within a specified time (deadline), usually a relatively short time. A real-time process is generally one that happens in defined ...
evidence. This can come from a
longitudinal study A longitudinal study (or longitudinal survey, or panel study) is a research design that involves repeated observations of the same variables (e.g., people) over short or long periods of time (i.e., uses longitudinal data). It is often a type of ob ...
, which tracks the same people.


Age-grading in different age groups


Children/adolescents

When children first learn their language, they learn language in the way of their mother and/or primary caretakers. Vernacular reorganization occurs when children adopt a norm that is different from the one they have acquired. This happens in the preadolescent and adolescent years and is necessary for linguistic change to advance.Tagliamonte, Sali. ''Variationist Sociolinguistics: Change, Observation, Interpretation'' (pp 46-55, 83, 186-87, 248-75, 316-17, 340-41). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, 2012. Print. It is not uncommon for adolescents to have certain linguistic variation in their speech. Adolescence is a transitional period when children become adults and move away from their family identity to express themselves as individuals. During this period, age-segregated networks form, giving rise to age-graded variation. Adolescents tend to lead with an increased use of vernacular and linguistic variables to differentiate themselves from the adult population.Eckert, Penelope (1997). ''Age as a Sociolinguistic Variable''. In Coulmas, Florian d ''The Handbook of Sociolinguistics'' (pp. 151-67). Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1997. Print. When studies began to include younger age groups, researchers (e.g. Ash 1982; Cedergren 1973, 1988) discovered that the frequency of incoming linguistic changes is highest among 15- to 17-year-olds, but lower among 11- to 14-year-olds and people in their twenties. This is called the adolescent peak.


Adults

Adults have been shown to be more conservative in their use of linguistic variables. This has been attributed to the desired use of standard language which is used in the workplace. Adolescents tend to use more slang and swear words, but these features recede as they become adults. This phenomenon is known as sociolectal retrenchment.


Older adults/elderly

Although middle-aged adults have conservative speech as mentioned above, older adults tend to have less formal speech. Older women who have passed the years of childbearing shed some of the conservatism in their speech. Similarly, older men who are past the age of retirement and no longer in the workforce also have less formal speech because of reduced social pressure to conform.


Studies/examples


''Zed'' vs. ''zee''

J.K. Chambers cites an example from southern Ontario, Canada. The last letter of the English alphabet is called ''zed'' everywhere in the English-speaking world except for the United States, where it is known as ''zee''. Though the use of ''zee'' is stigmatized in southern Ontario, children learn it this way because of the alphabet song that rhymes ''zee'' with ''vee''. Surveys in southern Ontario show a higher proportion of young people who produce ''zee'' compared to older people. In a Toronto survey in 1979, two-thirds of the twelve-year-olds completed their recitation of the alphabet with ''zee'' but only 8 percent of adults did so. In 1991, when those twelve-year-olds were twenty-five, another survey showed that 39 percent of twenty- to twenty-five-year-olds said ''zee''. This pattern of declining use of ''zee'' repeats itself in succeeding generations in southern Ontario.


Glottal stops in Glasgow

In Scotland and Northern England, children’s use of the glottal stop in place of /t/ is described as “the most openly stigmatised feature.” The glottal stop can occur in any non-initial post- tonic position and excludes words like time and tide, since the /t/ is word-initial, and in words like ''pretend'' and ''patella'', where it is pre-tonic. Examples where a glottal stop can replace a /t/ include words like ''better'', ''city'', ''dirty'', ''football'', ''hitting'', and ''water''. Children in Scotland and Northern England soon learn that the use of the glottal stop is considered inferior to the use of /t/ and are taught to correct themselves from an early age. Variation between the glottal stop and /t/ is mostly seen within the middle class due to pressure from adults. This case study provides an illustration of what Labov has identified as a “principle of transmission” (Labov 2001:437). He explains that “at some stage of socialization...children learn that variants favored in informal speech are associated with lower social status in the wider community.” Because of this, there is pressure to stop the use of the glottal stop by adolescence. However, since they are class-insensitive, children cannot be stopped from learning the glottal stop variant during childhood.


Instant messaging variants

The use of instant messaging variants ''lol'', ''haha'', and ''hehe'' provides a contemporary example of age-grading among adolescents. As shown in the figure to the right, the use of ''lol'' declines systematically according to age, with the youngest people using it the most frequently. Use of the more conservative form ''haha'' increases according to age. Older adolescents become aware that ''lol'' is used by a younger group and consciously reduce their use of the form.


See also

*
Variation (linguistics) Variation is a characteristic of language: there is more than one way of saying the same thing. Speakers may vary pronunciation ( accent), word choice ( lexicon), or morphology and syntax (sometimes called "grammar"). But while the diversity of v ...
*
Apparent-time hypothesis The apparent-time hypothesis is a methodological construct in sociolinguistics whereby language change is studied by comparing the speech of individuals of different ages. If language change is taking place, the apparent-time hypothesis assumes that ...


References

{{Reflist Language varieties and styles Sociolinguistics